Abstract
Background and purpose
Research indicates that mirror therapy reduces phantom limb pain (PLP). Objectives were to determine when mirror therapy works in those who respond to treatment, the relevance of baseline PLP to when pain relief occurs, and what pain symptoms respond to mirror therapy.
Methods
Data from two independent cohorts with unilateral lower limb amputation were analyzed for this study (n = 33). Mirror therapy consisted of 15-min sessions in which amputees performed synchronous movements of the phantom and intact legs/feet. PLP was measured using a visual analogue scale and the Short-Form McGill Pain Questionnaire.
Results
The severity of PLP at the beginning of treatment predicted when pain relief occurred. Those with low baseline PLP experienced a reduction (p < 0.05) in PLP by session 7 of treatment, those with medium baseline PLP experienced pain relief by session 14 of treatment, and those with high baseline PLP experienced pain relief by session 21 of treatment. Mirror therapy reduced throbbing, shooting, stabbing, sharp, cramping, aching, tender, splitting, tiring/exhausting, and punishing-cruel pain symptoms.
Conclusion
The degree of PLP at baseline predicts when mirror therapy relieves pain.
Implications
This article indicates that the degree of baseline PLP affects when mirror therapy relieves pain: relief occurs by session 7 in patients with low PLP but by session 21 in patients with high PLP. Clinicians should anticipate slower pain relief in patients who begin treatment with high levels of pain.
1 Introduction
Since its initial documentation over 500 years ago (Ambroise Paré), phantom limb pain (PLP) – pain in a missing limb – has eluded effective treatment [1,2]. Theories of why phantom limb pain occurs include learned paralysis, the neuromatrix, and proprioceptive memory [3,4,5]. Numerous therapies have failed to reduce pain effectively in randomized clinical trials [2]. One exception is mirror therapy, which appears to be effective and without the side effects that typically accompany pharmaceuticals [6].
Ramachandran and Rogers-Ramachandran first described mirror therapy over 20 years ago [7]. The therapy stemmed from the theory of learned paralysis, which posits that after amputation the brain continues to transmit efferent motor commands to the limb, but because the limb is missing no afferent sensory signals return to confirm that the limb successfully moved [3,7]. Over time this mismatch tricks the brain into perceiving the limb as paralyzed, which in turn causes pain. Mirror therapy was developed to reverse this paralysis by creating the illusion that the limb responds to motor commands. In mirror therapy, a mirror is placed between the intact and amputated limb to generate the visual impression of two healthy limbs. The individual then attempts to move both limbs in synchrony while watching the reflection, thus creating visual feedback that the limb is moving in response to motor commands and thereby reversing learned paralysis [3,7]. In their case series, Ramachandran and Rogers-Ramachandran reported that mirror therapy created the illusion of successful movement of the missing limb in 6 of 10 individuals, which for some reduced pain [7]. Subsequent research further supports the efficacy of mirror therapy. A randomized, sham-controlled trial of 22 patients showed that mirror therapy reduced PLP after lower extremity amputation as compared to a covered mirror condition (performing movements in front of a mirror covered by opaque sheet) and mental visualization (imagining movements with the amputated limb) [6]. Furthermore, with the exception of two cases of brief emotional reactions in the mirror group upon seeing the reflected limb, the trial did not detect any adverse side effects of treatment.
In spite of this evidence supporting the efficacy and safety of mirror therapy, in a survey of over 200 individuals with amputation(s) only 34% had tried mirror therapy and of these individuals only 40% reported benefit (unpublished data). One potential reason why research on mirror therapy has yet to translate widely into clinical practice is that the treatment parameters remain undefined; there is no standard treatment protocol for mirror therapy. Moreover, it is unclear who will respond to treatment and how long it takes to see therapeutic benefit. The present paper seeks to elucidate when and how mirror therapy works to inform treatment parameters with the hopes of allowing this therapy to enter standard clinical practice. The key items of interest were the trajectory of pain relief using mirror therapy, time to pain relief, the relevance of baseline pain to treatment response, and what pain qualities (e.g., throbbing, cramping and shooting) respond to mirror therapy.
2 Methods
2.1 Participants
Data from two independent cohorts with unilateral lower limb amputation were analyzed for this study. This study was retrospective, thus sample size was not calculated but rather all relevant data from the two studies were used.
In the first cohort, participants were recruited from Walter Reed Army Medical Center, Washington, DC from March 2006 through January 2007. Inclusion criteria included the presence of phantom limb pain greater than 3/10 on a visual analogue scale at least 3 times a week; exclusion criteria included bilateral lower or bilateral upper limb amputation, known neurological disease or brain damage, history of vertebral disk disease/condition, sciatica, or radiculopathy, known uncontrolled systemic disease, concurrent participation in another investigational drug or study device for phantom limb pain or participation in the 30 days immediately prior to study enrollment, current Axis I or II diagnosis determined by a neurologist or psychiatrist in the 6 months prior to entry into the study. The study was registered on clinicaltrials.gov (NCT00662415) and received approval from the Walter Reed Army Medical Center Institutional Review Board. Informed consent was sought and granted for all research subjects prior to enrollment in the study. The results of this study cohort were previously published and the specific data used for these analyses come only from mirror therapy sessions from the participants [6].
In the second cohort, participants were recruited from 2008 through 2014 from Walter Reed Army Medical Center and Walter Reed National Military Medical Center, Bethesda, MD as well as from the community for a functional magnetic resonance imaging study examining the effects of mirror therapy on brain activation patterns. Inclusion criteria included the presence of phantom limb pain greater than 3/10 on a visual analogue scale at least 3 times a week; exclusion criteria included multiple limb amputation, cause of amputation being diabetes or vascular claudication, pending revision surgeries, presence of embedded metallic shrapnel or other metal not compatible with MRI scanning, presence of traumatic brain injury, known neurological disease or brain damage, or history of vertebral disk disease/condition, sciatica, or radiculopathy, known uncontrolled systemic disease, concurrent participation in another investigational drug or study device for phantom limb pain or participation in the 30 days immediately prior to study enrollment, current Axis I or II diagnosis determined by a neurologist or psychiatrist in the 6 months prior to entry into the study, and pregnancy. The study was registered on clinicaltrials.gov (NCT00623818) and received approval from the respective Institutional Review Boards of Walter Reed and the National Institutes of Health. Informed consent was sought and granted for all research subjects prior to study enrollment. The results of this study have not yet been published.
2.2 Treatment
For both cohorts standard mirror therapy consisted of approximately 4 weeks of therapy sessions for 5 days a week, although treatment length and number of days/week varied depending on scheduling. Therapy sessions consisted of three different exercises, each lasting 5 min to total 15 min of therapy per day. Subjects flexed and extended the ankle (“as if stepping on the gas pedal of a car”), moved the foot from side to side (“windshield wiper”), and rotated the foot in a circle (“as if drawing a circle with your toes”), and for those with above knee amputation, flexion and extension of the leg at the knee (additional 5 min). At the beginning of each therapy session subjects were instructed to move the intact limb slowly to allow the phantom limb to move at the same pace. In addition subjects were instructed to move the phantom only as much as they could if range of movement was limited and to gradually increase the range of motion with each treatment session. Treatment was either conducted independently (participants followed instructions on their own) or directly observed by an investigator.
2.3 Outcome measures
2.3.1 Visual analogue scale
The visual analogue scale (VAS) is widely used in both clinical and research settings to measure pain. The VAS has been shown to be reliable, internally consistent, and sensitive to treatment [8,9]. The VAS in both studies consisted of a 100-mm horizontal line with two endpoints which were labelled “no pain” (far left) and “worst pain someone could ever experience” (far right). Subjects were given the following instructions: “Present Pain Intensity (PPI) – Visual Analogue Scale (VAS). Make a tick mark along the scale below that represents the phantom limb pain experienced over the last 24 hours”.
2.3.2 Short-Form McGill Pain Questionnaire
The Short-Form McGill Pain Questionnaire consists of 15 pain descriptors rated on a scale of 0 (corresponding to none) to 3 (corresponding to severe). The Short-Form McGill Pain Questionnaire has been shown to produce similar scores as the standard McGill Pain Questionnaire and be sensitive to treatment [10].
2.4 Effective versus ineffective treatment
Mirror therapy was deemed effective versus ineffective depending upon the decline in pain as measured by the VAS. For those with high (>60mm on the VAS) or medium (31–60 mm on the VAS) PLP at baseline, effective treatment was defined as a decline of at least 20 mm, which corresponds to the two point reduction specified by Farrar et al. [11]. For those with low (<30mm on the VAS) PLP at baseline, effective treatment was defined as a decrease of 50% over the course of treatment, a cut-off which is common in pharmaceutical trials since its recommendation by Moore et al. [12].
2.5 Analyses
2.5.1 Trajectory of pain relief with mirror therapy
The average pain trajectory across all patients was analyzed using polynomial regression of VAS scores versus treatment sessions. A logit transformation was used to fit the data given the lower limit of 0. Only participants for whom mirror therapy was effective (see definition under Section 2.4) were included in this analysis, as the question of interest was when pain relief occurred in cases of effective mirror therapy. In addition, a one-way ANOVA was conducted comparing the initial pain level to pain levels on sessions 2, 3, 4, and 5 of treatment to determine whether there is a PLP increase when beginning treatment. This was examined as anecdotally some patients have complained of increased PLP after starting mirror therapy.
2.5.2 Relevance of baseline pain level to treatment response
Data was divided into 3 categories according to baseline phantom limb pain as measured by the VAS: low (VAS score under 30 mm), medium (VAS score from 31 to 60 mm), and high (VAS score over 60 mm). A polynomial regression of VAS or McGill scores versus treatment sessions was used to look at the trajectory of pain relief for those with effective treatment (see definition under Section 2.4). A logit transformation was used to fit the data given the lower limit of 0 for both scales.
2.5.3 Time to pain relief
The data for effective treatment was then analyzed using a oneway ANOVA looking at sessions 1, 7, 14, and 21. These time points were chosen as the trajectory of pain relief indicated these to be the inflection points in treatment response. A Dunnett multiple comparisons test was used to compare pain levels on session 1 to pain levels on session 7, 14, and 21. We conducted independent analyses for each category of baseline pain (high, medium, and low).
2.5.4 PLP symptoms
The different pain qualities measured by the McGill questionnaire were examined individually to evaluate their specific responses to treatment. The average responses were analyzed using a quadratic regression. All subject data (i.e., those for whom mirror therapy was effective and those for whom it was ineffective) were included in this analysis.
2.5.5 Sensitivity analyses
Subjects with missing data were excluded from analyses requiring that data. To account for the possibility of bias due to missing data, sensitivity analyses were performed treating the VAS response for all subjects as observations in an intention to treat (ITT) model with a last observation carried forward (LOCF) approach to account for patients who did not complete all planned treatment sessions.
3 Results
3.1 Participants
Demographics are presented in Table 1 The participants for this study were predominately male (87.5%). The mean age for all study participants was 33.5 years (range: 19–60 years) and the mean time since amputation was 1.8 years (range: 0.05–21 years). In the first cohort, 32 subjects were screened, 22 of which were randomly assigned to one of three treatment groups (mirror, mental visualization, and covered mirror). The 10 subjects who were screened but not assigned to a treatment group declined to take part in the study due to lack of interest or inability to complete 1 month of treatment. Four subjects (3 from the mirror treatment group, 1 from the covered mirror treatment group) left the study during the first week due to inability to complete 1 month of treatment. Data from all subjects who received mirror therapy (21 subjects) were analyzed for this study. In the second cohort, 19 individuals with amputation were recruited; four of which were deemed ineligible based on the physical examination and followup information and two of which lost interest in the study prior to enrollment. One participant did not complete the study due to a personal issue. Additionally, data from two subjects were not used; the first subject did not submit the outcome measures of interest for this study and the second had an upper extremity amputation. Data from the remaining 10 subjects were analyzed for this study.
Demographics.
Age | Gender | Site of amputation | Time since amputation (years) |
---|---|---|---|
21 | M | Left Transfemoral | 0.3 |
25 | M | Right transtibial | 1 |
34 | M | Left transtibial | 0.2 |
33 | M | Left transfemoral | 0.1 |
20 | M | Right transfemoral | 0.2 |
20 | M | Right transtibial | 0.1 |
25 | M | Right transtibial | 1 |
38 | M | Left transtibial | 0.4 |
23 | M | Left transtibial | 0.1 |
19 | M | Left transtibial | 0.1 |
21 | M | Left transtibial | 0.1 |
31 | M | Right transfemoral | 0.05 |
39 | M | Left transfemoral | 1.7 |
31 | M | Left transtibial | 0.1 |
22 | M | Right transtibial | 0.2 |
53 | M | Right transfemoral | 0.2 |
20 | M | Right transfemoral | 1 |
22 | M | Right transtibial | 0.1 |
30 | M | Left transfemoral | 0.2 |
29 | M | Left transfemoral | 0.1 |
20 | M | Right transfemoral | 0.2 |
59 | F | Left transtibial | 3 |
21 | M | Left transtibial | 0.1 |
52 | M | Left transfemoral | 2 |
47 | M | Right transtibial | 21 |
75 | F | Right transfemoral | 5 |
36 | F | Left transfemoral | 0.25 |
60 | M | Right transfemoral | 15 |
45 | F | Right transtibial | 1 |
39 | M | Right transfemoral | 0.5 |
30 | M | Right knee disarticulation | 1 |
Study participants were not followed after termination of treatment, but the average number of treatment sessions was 19.3 (range: 3–40). One subject was missing data on the Short-Form McGill Pain Questionnaire; data on both variables was available for all other participants. There were two summary measures – the Short-Form McGill Pain Questionnaire and the VAS – measured over time.
3.2 Trajectory of pain relief with mirror therapy
In this combined cohort, mirror therapy was deemed effective for 27 of 31 (87%) subjects.The polynomial regression of VAS versus treatment sessions in these subjects showed a statistically significant decrease in pain over time (p< 0.0001). To allow for the bounded nature of the VAS scale, i.e., there is a lower limit of 0, a logit transformation was used to properly fit the data. As seen in Fig. 1, the average pain level of the group decreases substantially over the first 7 treatment sessions, plateaus from sessions 7 to 14, then again declines from session 14 onward. There was no statistically significant increase in pain upon commencement of treatment. A one-way ANOVA with Dunnet’s comparisons comparing the initial pain level to treatment 2, 3, 4 and 5 showed no significant difference (p = 0.761).

Model of phantom limb pain as measured by the VAS over time.
3.3 Relevance of baseline pain level to treatment response
A one-way ANOVA with Dunnet’s comparisons was used to compare the difference of the initial treatment pain levels to treatment sessions 7, 14, and 21. The polynomial regression of VAS versus treatment sessions indicated three different trajectories of pain relief for the categories of baseline phantom limb pain severities of low (VAS score under 30), medium (VAS score from 30 to 60), and high (VAS score over 60).
The low and medium initial VAS groups showed basically a continuous decrease (Figs. 2 and 3). The high initial VAS group showed an initial improvement, a levelling off, and then a final improvement (Fig. 4). Inspection of the individual patient trajectories showed that some of these patients improved rapidly, while others had an induction period of up to 15 sessions before therapy was effective. Consequently, the initial drop in the aggregate trajectory is related to those patients who improved immediately, while the final drop is from those who maintained high levels of pain through session 15.

Trajectory of pain relief for low baseline pain group.

Trajectory of pain relief for medium baseline pain group.

Trajectory of pain relief for high baseline pain group.
3.4 Time to pain relief
The Dunnett comparisons showed different findings for the low, medium, and high baseline pain groups. The low baseline pain group showed a significant difference in pain from sessions 1 to 7 (n = 8 distinct data measurements), sessions 1–14 (n = 6 distinct data measurements), and sessions 1–21 (n = 4 distinct data measurements; see Fig. 5). That is to say, for those with low initial PLP severity for whom treatment was effective, pain significantly dropped by session 7 of treatment. The medium baseline pain group showed a significant difference in pain from sessions 1 to 14 (n = 5 distinct data measurements) and sessions 1–21 (n = 4 distinct data measurements), but not from sessions 1 to 7 (n = 7 distinct data measurements; see Fig. 6). This indicates that for those with medium initial PLP for whom treatment was effective, pain significantly dropped by session 14 of treatment. The high baseline pain group showed a significant difference in pain from sessions 1 to 21 (n = 3 distinct data measurements), but not from sessions 1 to 7 (n = 7 distinct data measurements) or sessions 1–14 (n = 6 distinct data measurements; see Fig. 7). For those for with high initial PLP for whom treatment was effective, pain significantly dropped by session 21 of treatment.

Dunnett comparison for low baseline pain group.

Dunnett comparison for medium baseline pain group.

Dunnett comparison for high baseline pain group.
3.5 PLP symptoms
The quadratic regression looking at the responses of different pain qualities to treatment showed that some qualities showed a significant effect of treatment while others did not. Endorsement of the pain descriptors of throbbing, shooting, stabbing, sharp, cramping, aching, tender, splitting, tiring/exhausting, and punishing-cruel significantly decreased over the course of treatment (p < 0.05), whereas endorsement of the descriptors gnawing, hot/burning, heavy, sickening, fearful did not (Table 2).
Reduction in pain in each of the McGill pain characteristics.
Pain quality | p value for quadratic regression |
---|---|
Throbbing | <0.001[*] |
Shooting | <0.001[*] |
Stabbing | 0.0053[*] |
Sharp | <0.001[*] |
Cramping | 0.002[*] |
Gnawing | 0.0851 |
Hot/burning | 0.4226 |
Aching | 0.0342[*] |
Heavy | 0.5186 |
Tender | 0.0004[*] |
Splitting | 0.0053[*] |
Tiring/exhausting | 0.0192[*] |
Sickening | 0.7701 |
Fearful | 0.0626 |
Punishing-cruel | 0.0426[*] |
3.6 Sensitivity analysis
To evaluate the potential bias introduced by attrition, a sensitivity analysis was performed treating the VAS response for all subjects as observations in an intention to treat (ITT) model with a last observation carried forward (LOCF) approach to account for patients who did not complete all planned treatment sessions. This analysis showed minor differences in the model, and no differences in the overall conclusions. Similarly, a sensitivity analysis of the response from patients with effective treatment showed no deleterious data effects from attrition.
4 Discussion
This paper is the first to closely examine mirror therapy to determine when, after initiating treatment, an effect is seen and what pain sub-types this treatment works for. The first major finding is that PLP tends to decline rapidly during the first week of mirror therapy, plateau from sessions 7 to 14, and then declines from session 14 onward. However, this trajectory is influenced by the severity of PLP experienced at baseline. Those with low and medium levels of PLP at the beginning of treatment show essentially a continuous decline. There appears to be a dichotomy in the trajectory of pain relief for those initiating treatment with high levels of PLP. Some improve rapidly while others experience what appears to be an induction period of up to 15 sessions before a therapeutic effect is seen. Second, pain dropped by session 7 of treatment for those with low levels of PLP (p<0.05), by session 14 for those with medium levels of PLP (p < 0.05), and by session 21 for those with high levels of PLP (p<0.05). Finally, the study examined whether mirror therapy reduced certain pain qualities more than others, demonstrating that the pain qualities of throbbing, shooting, stabbing, sharp, cramping, aching, tender, splitting, tiring/exhausting, and punishing-cruel significantly (p<0.05) decreased over treatment, whereas endorsement of the descriptors gnawing, hot/burning, heavy, sickening, and fearful did not.
Selecting a treatment for PLP can be difficult given the number of treatment options available and how differently persons with major limb amputations can respond to the same treatment [2]. As such, determining early indicators of whether a therapy will work has the potential to tailor treatment to provide pain relief as quickly as possible or to avoid abandoning treatment before a point of efficacy can be expected. Mirror therapy appears to be a treatment that has a high efficacy rate combined with low cost and minimal side effects compared to other treatments for PLP [2,6]. However, as mirror therapy is not universally effective, it is important to recognize as soon as possible if it will ultimately prove ineffective for an individual, so that the healthcare provider can identify an alternate treatment. Our findings demonstrate that initial PLP severity affects how quickly mirror therapy can lead to pain reduction and may explain why some studies [13,14] did not report the same success rates reported by Chan et al. [6]. Most importantly, some patients with higher initial levels of PLP may take longer to respond to treatment than those with lower pain severity. Although anecdotally some of our patients reported transient increases in PLP after starting mirror therapy, upon detailed analysis we failed to find a statistically significant increase.
This study also examined how different pain qualities responded to mirror therapy. The pain qualities of throbbing, shooting, stabbing, sharp, cramping, aching, tender, splitting, tiring/exhausting and punishing-cruel responded to mirror therapy whereas there was not evidence that the other qualities did. Further work is necessary to examine why mirror therapy may affect certain sensations of pain and not others. One possibility would be that there are different mechanisms underlying phantom limb pain [2], that pain qualities correspond to different mechanisms [15], and that mirror therapy is thus only targeting certain pain generators and therefore only muting certain pain qualities. However, this is speculative given that pain qualities have yet to be linked to underlying patho-physiology.
Further research is necessary to substantiate and extend these findings. The first cohort was followed for up to 4 months per protocol after treatment ended although some study volunteers were seen incidentally up to 2 years later during routine longitudinal clinical evaluations. For those participants for whom PLP had decreased after 1 month of mirror therapy, all except 2 subjects reported that their PLP had resolved. The other 2 subjects reported that their PLP had returned following 2 months and that they had again used mirror therapy for 4 weeks with permanent resolution of PLP thereafter. Much longer follow-up is necessary to determine the long-term efficacy of mirror therapy, as well as the long-term efficacy of most other treatments for PLP. Together, examining predictors of treatment response and the mechanism of action of mirror therapy could contribute to developing a revised mechanism-based classification of PLP [2].
This study has several limitations. First, this is a post hoc analysis of a combined data set which constitutes a larger sample size than originally reported by Chan et al. [6]. Second, some participants did not complete the anticipated total number of 20 treatment sessions which could limit statistical power. However, the sensitivity analysis indicates that attrition did not bias study results. Third, since treatment was 5 days per week, data were analyzed by treatment sessions rather than day-to-day changes. Fourth, the participants consisted of a convenience sample that likely differs from the overall population of persons with amputation. Participants in the first cohort are primarily active duty military and thus tend to be younger, more physically active, and male. Participants from both cohorts lived in the Washington, DC metropolitan area; thus the sample is not nationally representative. Fifth, it is possible that some of the change in PLP seen over the course of the study was due to natural fluctuation in PLP [16] after amputation rather than mirror therapy. Lastly, to fully evaluate the utility of mirror therapy for treating PLP, a prospective comparison of this treatment with other treatments is necessary.
To conclude, this study is the first to examine mirror therapy in terms of when a treatment effect is seen and what pain qualities it treats. Results suggest that the majority of pain relief tends to occur over the first seven sessions of treatment, although initial PLP severity affects how quickly the response is seen, which has important practical implications for personalized treatment implementation.Additional research is necessary to confirm the importance of baseline pain levels and other characteristics as predictors of treatment response and to identify the mechanism of action of mirror therapy.
Highlights
The degree of PLP at baseline affects when mirror therapy relieves pain
Those with low baseline PLP tend to show pain relief by session 7 of treatment
Those with medium baseline PLP tend to show pain relief by session 14 of treatment
Those with high baseline PLP tend to show pain relief by session 21 of treatment
DOI of refers to article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.sjpain.2017.01.005.
Ethical issues: See Section 2.1
Conflicts of interest: The authors have no affiliations with or involvement in any organization or entity with financial interest or nonfinancial interest in the subject matter discussed in this manuscript.
The opinions or assertions contained herein are the private views of the authors and are not to be construed as official or reflecting the views of the Department of the Navy or the Department of Defense.
Acknowledgements
This study was supported in part by a David Mahoney grant from the Dana Foundation, the Intramural Research Program of the National Institutes of Health, the Military Amputee Research Program, and the Center for Rehabilitative Sciences Research (CRSR) at the Uniformed Services University of the Health Sciences
References
[1] Packard FR. Life and times of Ambroise Paré, 1510–1590: with a new translation of his apology and account of his journeys in divers places. PB Hoeberm Inc.; 1921.Search in Google Scholar
[2] Griffin SC, Tsao JW. A mechanism-based classification of phantom limb pain. Pain 2014;155:2236–42.10.1016/j.pain.2014.05.016Search in Google Scholar
[3] Ramachandran VS, Hirstein W. The perception of phantom limbs. Brain 1998;121:1603–30.10.1093/brain/121.9.1603Search in Google Scholar
[4] Melzack R. Phantom limbs and the concept of a neuromatrix. Trends Neurosci 1990;13:88–92.10.1016/0166-2236(90)90179-ESearch in Google Scholar
[5] Anderson-Barnes VC, McAuliffe C, Swanberg KM, Tsao JW. Phantom limb pain – a phenomenon of proprioceptive memory? Med Hypotheses 2009;73:555–8.10.1016/j.mehy.2009.05.038Search in Google Scholar
[6] Chan BL, Witt R, Charrow AP, Magee A, Howard R, Pasquina PF, Heilman KM, Tsao JW. Mirror therapy for phantom limb pain. N Engl J Med 2007;357:2206–7.10.1056/NEJMc071927Search in Google Scholar
[7] Ramachandran VS, Rogers-Ramachandran D. Synaesthesia in phantom limbs induced with mirrors. Proc R Soc Lond B: Biol Sci 1996;263:377–86.10.1098/rspb.1996.0058Search in Google Scholar
[8] Price DD, McGrath PA, Rafii A, Buckingham B. The validation of visual analogue scales as ratio scale measures for chronic and experimental pain. Pain 1983;17:45–56.10.1016/0304-3959(83)90126-4Search in Google Scholar
[9] Price DD, Von der Gruen A, Miller J, Rafii A, Price C. A psychophysical analysis of morphine analgesia. Pain 1985;22:261–9.10.1016/0304-3959(85)90026-0Search in Google Scholar
[10] Melzack R. The Short-Form McGill Pain Questionnaire. Pain 1987;30:191–7.10.1037/t04167-000Search in Google Scholar
[11] Farrar JT, Young JP, LaMoreaux L, Werth JL, Poole RM. Clinical importance of changes in pain intensity measured on an 11-point numerical pain rating scale. Pain 2001;94:149–58.10.1016/S0304-3959(01)00349-9Search in Google Scholar
[12] Moore A, Moore O, McQuay H, Gavaghan D. Deriving dichotomous outcome measures from continuous data in randomized controlled trials of analgesics: use of pain intensity and visual analogue scales. Pain 1997;69:311–5.10.1016/S0304-3959(96)03306-4Search in Google Scholar
[13] Brodie EE, Whyte A, Niven CA. Analgesia through the looking-glass? A randomized controlled trial investigating the effect of viewing a ‘virtual’ limb upon phantom limb pain, sensation, and movement. Eur J Pain 2007;11:428–36.10.1016/j.ejpain.2006.06.002Search in Google Scholar PubMed
[14] Hunter JP, Katz J, Davis KD. The effect of tactile and visual sensory inputs on phantom limb awareness. Brain 2003;126:579–89.10.1093/brain/awg054Search in Google Scholar PubMed
[15] Attal N, Fermanian C, Fermanian J, Lantéri-Minet M, Alchaar H, Bouhassira D. Neuropathic pain: are there distinct subtypes depending on the aetiology or anatomical lesion? Pain 2008;138:343–53.10.1016/j.pain.2008.01.006Search in Google Scholar PubMed
[16] Schley MT, Wilms P, Toepfner S, Schaller HP, Schmelz M, Konrad CJ, Birbaumer N. Painful and nonpainful phantom and stump sensations in acute traumatic amputees. J Trauma Acute Care Surg 2008;65:858–64.10.1097/TA.0b013e31812eed9eSearch in Google Scholar PubMed
© 2017 Scandinavian Association for the Study of Pain