Skip to content
Licensed Unlicensed Requires Authentication Published by De Gruyter July 1, 2017

The disruptive effects of pain on multitasking in a virtual errands task

David J. Moore and Anna S. Law

Abstract

Background and aims

Pain is known to have a disruptive effect on cognitive performance, but prior studies have used highly constrained laboratory tasks that lack ecological validity. In everyday life people are required to complete more complex sets of tasks, prioritising task completion and recalling lists of tasks which need to be completed, and these tasks continue to be attempted during episodes or states of pain. The present study therefore examined the impact of thermal induced pain on a simulated errand task.

Methods

Fifty-five healthy adults (36 female) performed the Edinburgh Virtual Errands Task (EVET) either during a painful thermal sensation or with no concurrent pain. Participants also completed the Experience of Cognitive Intrusion of Pain (ECIP) questionnaire to measure their self-reported cognitive impact of pain in general life.

Results

Participants who completed the EVET task in pain and who self-reported high intrusion of pain made significantly more errors than those who reported lower intrusion on the ECIP.

Conclusions

Findings here support the growing literature that suggests that pain has a significant impact on cognitive performance. Furthermore, these findings support the developing literature suggesting that this relationship is complex when considering real world cognition, and that self-report on the ECIP relates well to performance on a task designed to reflect the complexities of everyday living.

Implications

If extrapolated to chronic pain populations, these data suggest that pain during complex multitasking performance may have a significant impact on the number of errors made. For people highly vulnerable to cognitive intrusion by pain, this may result in errors such as selecting the wrong location or item to perform tasks, or forgetting to perform these tasks at the correct time. If these findings are shown to extend to chronic pain populations then occupational support to manage complex task performance, using for example diaries/electronic reminders, may help to improve everyday abilities.


DOI of refers to article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.sjpain.2017.04.001.



Research Centre for Brain and Behaviour, Liverpool John Moores University, Liverpool L3 3AF, UK.

  1. Ethical issues: All participants gave full informed consent to participate in this research and this research received ethical approval from the Liverpool John Moores University Research Ethics Committee (REF: 15/NSP/001).

  2. Conflict of interest: The authors have no conflict of interest in producing this manuscript.

Acknowledgements

The authors would like to thank Samra Anjum for her assistance with data collection during this research. No funding was given for this research.

References

[1] Eccleston C, Crombez G. Pain demands attention: a cognitive–affective model of the interruptive function of pain. Psychol Bull 1999;125:356–66.10.1037/0033-2909.125.3.356Search in Google Scholar

[2] Eccleston C. Chronic pain and distraction: an experimental investigation into the role of sustained and shifting attention in the processing of chronic persistent pain. Behav Res Ther 1995;33:391–405.10.1016/0005-7967(94)00057-QSearch in Google Scholar

[3] Legrain V, Crombez G, Mauraux A. Controlling attention to nociceptive stimuli with working memory. PLoS ONE 2011;6:e20926.10.1371/journal.pone.0020926Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

[4] Legrain V, Crombez G, Plaghki L, Mouraux A. Shielding cognition from nociception with working memory. Cortex 2013;49:1922–34.10.1016/j.cortex.2012.08.014Search in Google Scholar PubMed

[5] Buhle J, Wager TD. Performance-dependent inhibition of pain by an executive working memory task. Pain 2010;149:19–26.10.1016/j.pain.2009.10.027Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

[6] Veldhuijzen DS, Kenemans J, de Bruin C, Olivier B, Volkerts ER. Pain and attention: attentional disruption or distraction? J Pain 2006;7:11–20.10.1016/j.jpain.2005.06.003Search in Google Scholar PubMed

[7] Berryman C, Stanton TR, Bowering KJ, Tabor A, McFarlane A, Moseley GL. Evidence for working memory deficits in chronic pain: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Pain 2013;154:1181–96.10.1016/j.pain.2013.03.002Search in Google Scholar PubMed

[8] Berryman C, Stanton TR, Bowering KJ, Tabor A, McFarlane A, Moseley GL. Do people with chronic pain have impaired executive function? A meta-analytical review. Clin Psychol Rev 2014;34:563–79.10.1016/j.cpr.2014.08.003Search in Google Scholar PubMed

[9] Moriarty O, McGuire BE, Finn DP. The effect of pain on cognitive function: a review of clinical and preclinical research. Progr Neurobiol 2011;93:385–404.10.1016/j.pneurobio.2011.01.002Search in Google Scholar PubMed

[10] Moore DJ, Keogh E, Eccleston C. The interuptive effect of pain on attention. Q J Exp Psychol 2012;65:565–86.10.1080/17470218.2011.626865Search in Google Scholar PubMed

[11] Moore DJ, Keogh E, Eccleston C. The effect of threat on attentional interruption by pain. Pain 2013;154:82–8.10.1016/j.pain.2012.09.009Search in Google Scholar

[12] Moore DJ, Keogh E, Eccleston C. Headache impairs attentional performance. Pain 2013;154:1840–5.10.1016/j.pain.2013.06.006Search in Google Scholar

[13] Hamacher D, Hamacher D, Schega L. A cognitive dual task affects gait variability in patients suffering from chronic low back pain. Exp Brain Res 2014;232:3509–13.10.1007/s00221-014-4039-1Search in Google Scholar

[14] Shallice T, Burgess PW. Deficits in strategy application following frontal lobe damage in man. Brain 1991;114:727–41.10.1093/brain/114.2.727Search in Google Scholar

[15] Alderman N, Burgess PW, Knight C, Henman C. Ecological validity of a simplified version of the multiple errands shopping test. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2003;9:31–44.10.1017/S1355617703910046Search in Google Scholar

[16] Rand D, Rukan SB-A, Weiss PL, Katz N. Validation of the Virtual MET as an assessment tool for executive functions. Neuropsychol Rehabil 2009;19:583–602.10.1080/09602010802469074Search in Google Scholar

[17] Knight RG, Titov N, Crawford M. The effects of distraction on prospective remembering following traumatic brain injury assessed in a simulated naturalistic environment. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2006;12:8–16.10.1017/S1355617706060048Search in Google Scholar

[18] Keogh E, Moore DJ, Duggan GB, Payne SJ, Eccleston C. The disruptive effects of pain on complex cognitive performance and executive control. PLoS ONE 2013;8:e83272.10.1371/journal.pone.0083272Search in Google Scholar

[19] Roelofs J, Peters ML, McCracken L, Vlaeyen JW. The pain vigilance and awareness questionnaire (PVAQ): further psychometric evaluation in fibromyalgia and other chronic pain syndromes. Pain 2003;101:299–306.10.1016/S0304-3959(02)00338-XSearch in Google Scholar

[20] Attridge N, Crombez G, Van Ryckeghem D, Keogh E, Eccleston C. The experience of cognitive intrusion of pain: scale development and validation. Pain 2015;156:1978–90.10.1097/j.pain.0000000000000257Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

[21] Logie RH, Trawley S, Law A. Multitasking: multiple, domain-specific cognitive functions in a virtual environment. Mem Cogn 2011;39:1561–74.10.1037/e520602012-055Search in Google Scholar

[22] Moore DJ, Eccleston C, Keogh E. Does sex moderate the relationship between anxiety and pain? Psychol Health 2013;28:746–64.10.1080/08870446.2012.759222Search in Google Scholar

[23] Mounce C, Keogh E, Eccleston C. A principal components analysis of negative affect-related constructs relevant to pain: evidence for a three component structure. J Pain 2010;11:710–7.10.1016/j.jpain.2009.10.014Search in Google Scholar

[24] McCracken LM, Zayfert C, Gross RT. The pain anxiety symptoms scale: development and validation of a scale to measure fear of pain. Pain 1992;50:67–73.10.1016/0304-3959(92)90113-PSearch in Google Scholar

[25] Coons MJ, Hadjistavropoulos HD, Asmundson GJG. Factor structure and psychometric properties of the Pain Anxiety Symptoms Scale-20 in a community physiotherapy clinic sample. Eur J Pain 2004;8:511–6.10.1016/j.ejpain.2003.11.018Search in Google Scholar PubMed

[26] Abrams MP, Carleton RN, Asmundson GJG. An exploration of the psychometric properties of the PASS-20 with a nonclinical sample. J Pain 2007;8:879–86.10.1016/j.jpain.2007.06.004Search in Google Scholar PubMed

[27] Taylor S, Zvolensky MJ, Cox BJ, Deacon B, Heimberg RG, Ledley DR, Abramowitz JS, Holaway RM, Sadin B, Stewart SH, Coles M, Eng W, Daly ES, Arrindell WA, Bouvard M, Cardenas SJ. Robust dimensions of anxiety sensitivity: development and initial validation of the Anxiety Sensitivity Index-3. Psychol Assess 2007;19:176–88.10.1037/1040-3590.19.2.176Search in Google Scholar PubMed

[28] McNeil DW, Rainwater AJ. Development of the Fear of Pain Questionnaire-III. J Behav Med 1998;21:389–410.10.1023/A:1018782831217Search in Google Scholar

[29] Sullivan MJL, Bishop SR, Pivik J. The Pain Catastrophizing Scale: development and validation. Psychol Assess 1995;7:524–32.10.1037/1040-3590.7.4.524Search in Google Scholar

[30] Lovibond SH, Lovibond PF. Manual for the depression anxiety and stress scales. Sydney: Psychology Foundation; 1995.10.1037/t01004-000Search in Google Scholar

[31] Henry JD, Crawford JR. The short-form version of the Depression Anxiety Stress Scales (DASS-21): construct validity and normative data in a large non-clinical sample. Br J Clin Psychol 2005;44:227–39.10.1348/014466505X29657Search in Google Scholar

[32] Antony MM, Bieling PJ, Cox BJ, Enns MW, Swinson RP. Psychometric properties of the 42-item and 21-item versions of the Depression Anxiety Stress Scales in clinical groups and a community sample. Washington, DC, ETATS-UNIS: American Psychological Association; 1998.10.1037/1040-3590.10.2.176Search in Google Scholar

[33] Clark-Carter D. Quantitative psychological research: a student’s handbook. Hove, UK: Psychology Press; 2004.10.4324/9780203462119Search in Google Scholar

[34] Stevens J. Applied multivariate statistics in the social sciences. 3rd ed. New Jersey: LEA; 1996.Search in Google Scholar

[35] Poole HM, Wade K, Fairclough S, Elves S. Gaming in the clinic: effects of game demand on pain experience in adult burns patients. In: International Association for the Study of Pain (IASP) World Congress. 2014.Search in Google Scholar

[36] Peters ML, Vlaeyen JWS, Kunnen AMW. Is pain-related fear a predictor of somatosensory hypervigilance in chronic low back pain patients? Behav Res Ther 2002;40:85–103.10.1016/S0005-7967(01)00005-5Search in Google Scholar

[37] Van Damme S, Crombez G, Eccleston C. Disengagement from pain: the role of catastrophic thinking about pain. Pain 2004;107:70–6.10.1016/j.pain.2003.09.023Search in Google Scholar

[38] Crombez G, Eccleston C, Van Damme S, Vlaeyen JWS, Karoly P. The Fear Avoidance Model of chronic pain: the next generation. Clin J Pain 2012;28:475–83.10.1097/AJP.0b013e3182385392Search in Google Scholar

[39] Keogh E, Cavill R, Moore DJ, Eccleston C. The effects of menstrual-related pain on attentional interference. Pain 2014;155:821–7.10.1016/j.pain.2014.01.021Search in Google Scholar

[40] Edens JL, Gil KM. Experimental induction of pain: utility in the study of clinical pain. Behav Ther 1995;26:197–216.10.1016/S0005-7894(05)80102-9Search in Google Scholar

[41] Moore DJ, Keogh E, Crombez G, Eccleston C. Methods for studying naturally occurring human pain and their analogues. Pain 2013;154:190–9.10.1016/j.pain.2012.07.016Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

Received: 2016-08-26
Revised: 2017-02-07
Accepted: 2017-02-21
Published Online: 2017-07-01
Published in Print: 2017-07-01

© 2017 Scandinavian Association for the Study of Pain