Skip to content
Licensed Unlicensed Requires Authentication Published by De Gruyter Mouton December 24, 1999

A Cross-Language Comparison of Speaking Rate Effects on the Production of Voice Onset Time in English and Spanish

  • Joël Magloire and Kerry P. Green
From the journal Phonetica

Abstract

Of interest in the current study was how voice inset time (VOT) was influenced by changes in speaking rate across Spanish and English. Three groups of subjects (English monolinguals, Spanish monolinguals and early Spanish-English bilinguals) produced sentences containing voiced and voiceless bilabial stops at different speaking rates. As in previous research, English monolinguals showed rate-dependent effects on their VOT productions: VOT increased as speaking rate decreased. Spanish monolinguals showed a large effect of speaking rate on the duration of prevoicing of the voiced stops. However, they showed only a small effect of rate on the VOT of their voiceless stops. The bilinguals produced VOT values in each language that were nearly identical to their monolingual counterparts. The results from this study indicate that short-lag stops experience minimal variation as a function of speaking rate regardless of the other contrasting phonetic categories within a particular language. In addition, early bilinguals showed evidence of separate representations for voiced and voiceless stops for English and Spanish.


verified


References

1 Baum, S.R.; Ryan, L.: Rate of speech effects in aphasia: VOT. Brain Lang. 44: 431–445 (1993).10.1006/brln.1993.1026Search in Google Scholar

2 Caramazza, A.; Yeni-Komshian, G.H.; Zurif, E.B.; Carbone, E.: The acquisition of a new phonological contrast: the case of stop consonants in French-English bilinguals. J. acoust. Soc. Am. 54: 421–428 (1973).10.1121/1.1913594Search in Google Scholar

3 Chomsky, N.; Halle, M.: The sound pattern of English (Harper & Row, New York 1968).Search in Google Scholar

4 Cooper, W.E.: The development of speech timing; in Segalowitz, Gruber, Language development and neurological theory, pp. 357–373 (Academic Press, New York 1977).10.1016/B978-0-12-635650-2.50028-5Search in Google Scholar

5 Epstein, W.; Broota, K.D.: Automatic and attentional components in perception of size-at-a-distance. Perception Psychophysics 40: 256–262 (1986).10.3758/BF03211505Search in Google Scholar

6 Flege, J.E.: Age of learning affects the authenticity of voice onset time in stop consonants produced in a second language. J. acoust. Soc. Am. 89: 395–411 (1991).10.1121/1.400473Search in Google Scholar

7 Flege, J.E.: Speech learning in a second language; in Ferguson, Menn, Stoel-Gammon, Phonological development: models, research and applications, pp. 565–604 (York Press, Timonium 1992).Search in Google Scholar

8 Flege, J.E.: Second language speech learning theory: findings and problems; in Strange, Speech perception and linguistic experience, pp. 233–272 (York Press, Timonium 1995).Search in Google Scholar

9 Flege, J.E.; Eefting, W.: Production and perception of English stops by native Spanish speakers. J. Phonet. 15: 67–83 (1987).10.1016/S0095-4470(19)30538-8Search in Google Scholar

10 Flege, J.E.; Hillenbrand, J.: Limits on phonetic accuracy in foreign language speech production. J. acoust. Soc. Am. 76: 708–721 (1984).10.1121/1.391257Search in Google Scholar

11 Flege, J.E.; Schmidt, A.M.; Wharton, G.: Age of learning affects rate-dependent processing of stops in a second language. Phonetica 53: 143–161 (1996).10.1159/000262195Search in Google Scholar

12 Grosjean, F.: Another view of bilinguism; in Harris, Cognitive processing in bilinguals, pp. 51–62 (Elsevier/North Holland, Amsterdam 1992).10.1016/S0166-4115(08)61487-9Search in Google Scholar

13 Grosjean, F.: Individual bilingualism; in The encyclopedia of language and linguistics (Pergamon Press, Oxford 1994).Search in Google Scholar

14 Grosjean, F.: Studying bilinguals: methodological and conceptual issues. Bilingualism: language and cognition (in press).Search in Google Scholar

15 Harris, J.W.: Spanish phonology (MIT Press, Cambridge 1969).Search in Google Scholar

16 Hazan, V.L.; Boulakia, G.: Perception and production of a voicing contrast by French-English bilinguals. Lang. Speech 36: 17–38 (1993).10.1177/002383099303600102Search in Google Scholar

17 Keating, P.: Phonetic and phonological representation of stop consonant voicing. Language 60: 286–319 (1984).10.2307/413642Search in Google Scholar

18 Kessinger, R.H.; Blumstein, S.E.: Effects of speaking rate on voice-onset time in Thai, French and English. J. Phonet. 25: 143–168 (1997).10.1006/jpho.1996.0039Search in Google Scholar

19 Kewley-Port, D.; Preston, M.S.: Early typical stop production: a voice onset analysis. J. Phonet. 2: 195–210 (1974).10.1016/S0095-4470(19)31270-7Search in Google Scholar

20 Labov, W.: The social stratification of English in New York City (1966).Search in Google Scholar

21 Lisker, L.; Abramson, A.S.: A cross-language study of voicing in initial stops: acoustical measurements. Word 20: 384–422 (1964).10.1080/00437956.1964.11659830Search in Google Scholar

22 Lisker, L.; Abramson, A.S.: The voicing dimension: some experiments in comparative phonetics. Proc. 6th Int. Conf. Phonet. Sci. (Academia, Prague 1970).Search in Google Scholar

23 Löfqvist, A.: Interarticulator programming in stop production. J. Phonet. 8: 475–490 (1980).10.1016/S0095-4470(19)31502-5Search in Google Scholar

24 Löfqvist, A.: Acoustic and aerodynamic effects of interarticulator timing in voiceless consonants. Lang. Speech 35: 15–28 (1992).10.1177/002383099203500203Search in Google Scholar

25 Löfqvist, A.: Laryngeal mechanisms and interarticulator timing in voiceless consonant production; in Bell-Berti, Raphael, Producing speech: contemporary issues, pp. 99–116 (American Institute for Physics Press, New York 1995).Search in Google Scholar

26 Mack, M.: Consonant and vowel perception and production: early English-French bilingual and English monolinguals. Perception Psychophysics 46: 187–200 (1989).10.3758/BF03204982Search in Google Scholar

27 Macken, M.A.: Aspects of teh acquisition of stop systems: a cross-linguistic perspective; in Yeni-Komshian, Kavanagh, Ferguson, Child phonology, vol. 1: Production, pp. 143–168 (Academic Press, New York 1981). Macken, M.A.; Barton, D.: The acquisition of the voicing contrast in Spanish: a phonetic and phonological study of word-initial stop consonants. J. Child Lang. 7: 433–458 (1980).Search in Google Scholar

28 McNeil, M.R.; Kent, R.D.: Motoric characteristics of adult aphasic and apraxic speakers; in Hammond, Cerebral control of speech and limb movements, pp. 349–386 (Elsevier/North Holland, Amsterdam 1990).10.1016/S0166-4115(08)60655-XSearch in Google Scholar

29 Miller, J.L.; Baer, T.: Some effects of speaking rate on the production of /b/ and /w/. J. acoust. Soc. Am. 73: 1751–1755 (1983).10.1121/1.389399Search in Google Scholar

30 Miller, J.L.; Green, K.P.; Reeves, A.: Speaking rate and segments: a look at the relation between speech production and speech perception for the voicing contrast. Phonetica 43: 106–115 (1986).10.1159/000261764Search in Google Scholar

31 Miller, J.L.; Grosjean, F.; Lomanto C.: Articulation and its variability in spontaneous speech: a reanalysis and some implications. Phonetica 41: 215–225 (1984).10.1159/000261728Search in Google Scholar

32 Miller, J.L.; Volaitis, L.E.: Effects of speaking rate on perceptual structure of a phonetic category. Perception Psychophysics 46: 505–512 (1989).10.3758/BF03208147Search in Google Scholar

33 Obler, L.K.: The parsimonious bilingual; in Obler, Menn, Exceptional language and linguistics, pp. 339–346 (Academia, New York 1982).Search in Google Scholar

34 Peterson, G.E.; Barney, H.L.: Control methods used in a study of vowels. J. acoust. Soc. Am. 24: 175–184 (1952).10.1121/1.1906875Search in Google Scholar

35 Pind, J.: Speaking rate, VOT, and quantity: the search for higher order invariants for two Icelandic speech cues. Perception Psychophysics 57: 291–304 (1995).Search in Google Scholar

36 Pisoni, D.B.: Effects of alcohol on acoustic-phonetic properties of speech: perceptual and acoustic analyses. Alcoholism clin. exp. Res. 13: 577–587 (1989).10.1111/j.1530-0277.1989.tb00381.xSearch in Google Scholar

37 Raphael, L.J.; Tobin, Y.; Faber, A.; Most, T.; Kollia, H.B.; Milstein, D.: Intermediate values of voice onset time; in Bell-Berti, Raphael, Producing speech: contemporary issues, pp. 117–128 (American Institute for Physics Press, New York 1995).Search in Google Scholar

38 Raphael, L.J.; Tobin, Y.; Most, T.: Atypical VOT categories in Hebrew and Spanish. J. acoust. Soc. 74: S89A (1983).10.1121/1.2021206Search in Google Scholar

39 Schmidt, A.M.; Flege, J.E.: Effects of speaking rate changes on native and nonnative speech production. Phonetica 52: 41–54 (1995).10.1159/000262028Search in Google Scholar

40 Schmidt, A.M.; Flege, J.E.: Speaking rate effects on stops produced by Spanish and English monolinguals and Spanish/English bilinguals. Phonetica 53: 162–179 (1996).10.1159/000262196Search in Google Scholar

41 Summerfield, Q.: Aerodynamics versus mechanics in the control of voicing onset in consonant-vowel syllables. Speech Perception, No. 4 (Department of Psychology, Queen’s University, Belfast 1975).Search in Google Scholar

42 Summerfield, Q.: Articulatory rate and perceptual constancy in phonetic perception. J. exp. Psychol. hum. Perception Performance 7: 1074–1095 (1981).10.1037/0096-1523.7.5.1074Search in Google Scholar

43 Volaitis, L.E.; Miller, J.L.: Phonetic prototypes: influence of place of articulation and speaking rate on the internal structure of voicing categories. J. acoust. Soc. Am. 92: 723–735 (1992).10.1121/1.403997Search in Google Scholar

44 Weismer, G.: Sensitivity of voice-onset time (VOT) measures to certain segmental features in speech production. J. Phonet. 7: 197–204 (1979).10.1016/S0095-4470(19)31041-1Search in Google Scholar

45 Weismer, G.: Control of the voicing distinction for intervocalic stops and fricatives: some data and theoretical considerations. J. Phonet. 8: 427–438 (1980).10.1016/S0095-4470(19)31498-6Search in Google Scholar

46 Williams, L.: The voicing contrast in Spanish. J. Phonet. 5: 169–184 (1977a).10.1016/S0095-4470(19)31127-1Search in Google Scholar

47 Williams, L.: The perception of stop consonant voicing by Spanish-English bilinguals. Perception Psychophysics 21: 289–297 (1977b).10.3758/BF03199477Search in Google Scholar

48 Williams, L.: The modification of speech perception and production in second language learning. Perception Psychophysics 26: 95–104 (1979).10.3758/BF03208301Search in Google Scholar

49 Williams, L.: Phonetic variation as a function of second language learning; in Yeni-Komshian, Kavanagh, Ferguson, Child phonology, vol. 2: Perception, pp. 185–215 (Academic Press, New York 1980).Search in Google Scholar

50 Zampini, M.L.: Voiced stop spirantization in the ESL speech of native speakers of Spanish. Appl. Psycholinguistics 17: 335–354 (1996).10.1017/S0142716400007979Search in Google Scholar

Published Online: 1999-12-24
Published in Print: 1999-12-01

© 2000 S. Karger AG, Basel

Downloaded on 1.6.2023 from https://www.degruyter.com/document/doi/10.1159/000028449/html
Scroll to top button