Skip to content
BY 4.0 license Open Access Published by De Gruyter Mouton April 18, 2022

Changes in the midst of a construction network: a diachronic construction grammar approach to complex prepositions denoting internal location

Guillaume Desagulier ORCID logo
From the journal Cognitive Linguistics

Abstract

Linguists have debated whether complex prepositions deserve a constituent status, but none have proposed a dynamic model that can both predict what construal a given pattern imposes and account for the emergence of non-spatial readings. This paper reframes the debate on constituency as a justification of the constructional status of complex prepositional patterns from a historical perspective. It focuses on the Prep NP IL of NP lm construction, which denotes a relation of internal location between a located entity (a trajector) and a reference entity (a landmark). Four subschemas of the Internal Location construction are examined: middle cxn (in the middle of), center cxn (in/at the center of), heart cxn (in/at the heart of), and midst cxn (in the midst of). All occurrences are extracted from the COHA, along with their co-occurring landmark NPs. Using vocabulary growth curves, all patterns are shown to be productive over the whole period covered by the corpus, although at different levels. Using word2vec, a semantic vector space with the landmark collocates of each pattern is made. Curves indexed on association scores are plotted to see how densely semantic areas have been populated across four consecutive periods: 1810s–1860s, 1870s–1910s, 1920s–1970s, and 1980s–2000s. Two divisions of labor have emerged. midst cxn and heart cxn are in complementary distribution and operate mostly at the level of abstract locations whereas middle cxn and center cxn are in parallel distribution and operate at the level of concrete locations.

1 Introduction

Construction Grammar (CxG) holds that linguistic knowledge consists of a network of symbolic units, i.e. form–meaning pairings known as constructions. Although these have been recognized to exist at various levels of complexity, schematicity, and analyzability in the network (Goldberg 2003: 220), pride of place has been given to the study of language phenomena that are “often idiosyncratic, unpredictable and riddled with exceptions” (Hilpert 2021: 37). If it is indeed the case that “the network of constructions captures our grammatical knowledge of language in toto” (Goldberg 2006: 18), CxG must confirm its added value by accounting for patterns that are less idiomatic and more analyzable in terms of general syntactic mechanisms.

Complex prepositions (e.g. ahead of, in spite of) are a good case in point. Although very common in English (Hoffmann 2005; Leitner 2011), their status is problematic with respect to form and meaning. With respect to form, their constituency has been a matter of debate. On the one hand, formal approaches deny complex prepositions a unit status on the grounds that the string of contiguous words can always be broken by at least one manipulation at the level of underlying syntactic representations (Huddleston 1988; Huddleston and Pullum 2002; Seppänen et al. 1994). Whether such manipulations are attested is never envisaged because deep-parsing effects are considered independent from local constraints and immutable. On the other hand, descriptive grammars subscribe to a continuum ranging from free assemblies to fixed units (Quirk and Mulholland 1964; Quirk et al. 1985; Biber et al. 1999). This idea of a continuum has received empirical validation from corpus-based and usage-based studies (Hoffmann 2005; Beckner and Bybee 2009).

With respect to meaning, most complex prepositions commonly extend beyond the expression of spatial location (Langacker 2010; Tyler and Evans 2003). Gréa (2017) studies five prepositional patterns in French (parmi ‘among’, au centre de ‘at the centre of’, au milieu de ‘in the middle of’, au cœur de ‘at the heart of’, and au sein de ‘at the breast/bosom of’). They denote a relationship of inclusion between a trajector (tr), i.e. a located entity, and a landmark (lm), i.e. a reference entity that is the secondary focal participant (Langacker 1987: 225–228). Gréa shows that each construes inclusion in its own way. Strikingly, none of the criteria that distinguish them is geometrical. What is decisive is not so much that the trajector is equidistant from the boundaries of the landmark but a system of interacting parameters: the landmark’s internal plurality, its boundedness, the magnitude of its referent, and the degree of functional dependence between the trajector and the landmark. Although Gréa develops a model that can predict which semantic facets of a given landmark NP are activated by a given preposition, the model itself is static and it is not clear what drives the semantic change and how the division of labor among the patterns has unfolded over time.

The benefit of a constructional approach to complex prepositions is that it allows us to address the formal and functional issues identified above within a unified theoretical framework. More precisely, it reframes the issues of constituency, meaning change, and division of labor as a justification of constructional status from a historical perspective, along the lines laid out by the Construction Grammar framework (CxG) (Goldberg 1995, 2006, 2009) and its most recent offshoot: Diachronic Construction Grammar (DCxG) (Bardal et al. 2015; Traugott and Trousdale 2013).

This paper investigates four complex-prepositional patterns that denote a relation of internal location between a trajector and a landmark: in the middle of NP, in/at the center of NP, in/at the heart of NP, and in the midst of NP. They are exemplified below:[1]

(1)
He stops suddenly in the middle of the stage and seems to consider. (1815-FIC)[2]
(2)
Marvin walked to the chalk mark in the center of the ring. (1934-FIC)
(3)
(…) we were in the heart of Norwalk. (1827-FIC)
(4)
We see St Eustace praying in the midst of the river. (1980-FIC)

My first goal is to justify the unit status of these complex prepositions from a CxG perspective. Four corresponding constructions are hypothesized to exist: middle cxn, center cxn, heart cxn, and midst cxn. The consistent higher-order paradigm that they are assumed to be part of is a multi-level construction network headed by a more schematic construction, namely the Internal Location construction (henceforth the IL construction). Although there is no set list of criteria for ‘constructionhood’,[3] the patterns will nevertheless be recognized to count as constructions if they are productive, schematic, and impose a specific construal upon the trajector/landmark relation, based on a systematic inspection of their most distinctive landmark collocates in the COHA.

My second goal is to show that, like all construction networks, the IL construction network is dynamic and subject to internal reorganization and optimization. To keep track of the shifting distributions of middle cxn, center cxn, heart cxn, and midst cxn over time, their collocational preferences will be compared across four periods in the COHA (Davies 2010): 1810s–1860s, 1870s–1910s, 1920s–1970s, and 1980s–2000s (the periodization will be justified in Section 3). The collocates will be mapped onto a reference semantic vector space using a predictive distributional semantic model (word2vec’s SGNS). Frequency-based contour plots will be added onto this reference space for each period and each subschema. The resulting dynamic visualizations will help detect cases of schematization and show how the network optimizes the distribution of its subschematic nodes to cover all the relevant aspects of internal location.

This paper does not address other markers of internal location, some of which fit the Prep NP IL of NP lm constructional template (in the core/crux of), whereas others fulfill similar functions but display a different morphosyntax, such as the simple prepositions amid, amidst, among, and amongst. Regarding the first kind of markers, they were excluded from a quantitative treatment because they are too rare in the COHA (N core  = 134, N crux  = 13). Regarding the second kind of markers, they are assumed to part of a distinct sub-network which, arguably, is linked to the functional stratum of the IL construction.[4] Future work is therefore needed to understand how this sub-network connects to the more schematic IL construction network. This paper does not attempt either to model the respective contributions of the subschemas to the reinforcement of the mother node, which Hilpert (2015) calls ‘the upward strengthening hypothesis’. Research is underway to test this hypothesis.

The paper is structured as follows. Section 2 reviews the terms of the debate with respect to constituency. The four prepositional patterns count as constructions because of a conflation of formal and functional factors. Section 3 presents the data and, for each subschema, compares frequency of use to productivity. Section 4 explains the methodology underlying the design of diachronic vector spaces, namely the combination of a distributional semantic model and frequency-based contour lines. Such maps show that the most productive nodes in the network, i.e. midst cxn and heart cxn, are also the most schematic. They have ended up sharing the task of locating trajectors within abstract locations, whereas middle cxn and center cxn locate trajectors within concrete locations. Section 5 provides a qualitative assessment of the data, showing that the four subschemas are not strictly compositional and can no longer be analyzed under the sole perspective of spatial criteria. Section 6 summarizes the results and sketches the IL construction network. It concludes with a description of the dynamics at work in the vertical and horizontal dimensions of the network.

2 Complex prepositions as constructions

2.1 The debate around complex prepositions

Hoffmann (2005: 26) observes that complex prepositions are mentioned in 19th- and 20th-century grammars. Such units go by different names: ‘phrasal prepositions’ (Earle 1873), ‘prepositional formulæ’ (Mätzner 1873), ‘compound prepositions’, or ‘group prepositions’ (Sweet 1892). These grammarians recognize that some Prep1 NP Prep2 sequences function like prepositions. Yet, none goes as far as to grant these sequences full prepositional status. One reason given by Sweet (1892: 134–135) is the apparent mismatch between form and function: syntactically, Prep2 governs the following noun; functionally, the noun is governed by the whole group.

Quirk et al. (1985: 671) concede the following: “[i]n the strictest definition, a complex preposition is a sequence that is indivisible both in terms of syntax and in terms of meaning”. However, they add that, in usage, whether complex prepositions count as units or free assemblies is a matter of gradience: “there is no absolute distinction between complex prepositions and constructions which can be varied, abbreviated, and extended according to the normal rules of syntax”. Accordingly, Quirk et al. develop a scale of cohesiveness that comprises “nine indicators of syntactic separateness”:

  1. Prep2 can be varied;

  2. the noun can be varied as between singular and plural;

  3. the noun can be varied in respect to determiners;

  4. Prep1 can be varied;

  5. Prep2 + complement can be replaced by a possessive pronoun;

  6. Prep2 + complement can be omitted;

  7. Prep2 + complement can be replaced by a demonstrative;

  8. the noun can be replaced by nouns of related meaning;

  9. the noun can be freely premodified by adjectives.

The most cohesive sequences are those that behave exactly like simple prepositions, e.g. in spite of. The least cohesive sequences are those whose constituents behave independently, e.g. on the shelf (by the door). In the same line of thought, Biber et al. (1999: 76) go as far as to say that the degrees of variability make it impossible to decide where to draw the line between free combinations and units.

Not all linguists agree with gradient constituency. Huddleston (1988: 126) characterizes Prep1 NP Prep2 sequences in terms of a “mismatch” between the lexicon and grammar due to lexicalization. For example, in for the sake of the premier, although the four words for the sake of belong together lexically, the immediate grammatical constituents are for + the sake of the premier. This is further evidenced by the possibility of the genitive alternant: for the premier’s sake. Huddleston does not treat such sequences as complex prepositions because lexical chunking does not override grammatical parsing in his approach.

Huddleston and Pullum (2002) group Prep1 NP Prep2 sequences under the heading “Idiomatic and fossilised expressions headed by a preposition”.[5] Such sequences differ from free expressions because they are characterized by varying degrees of idiomaticity (i.e. non-compositionality) and fossilization. Unlike free expressions, some instances of Prep1 (Article) N1 Prep2 X do not allow the whole range of syntactic manipulations that are permitted with free expressions. Some of these manipulations overlap with Quirk et al.’s nine indicators of syntactic separateness: occurrence without Prep2, omission of Prep2 + X, modification of N1, number change in N1, and determiner change. They propose additional manipulations: genitive alternation, coordination of N1, coordination of Prep2 + X, fronting of Prep2 + X. These constituency tests are meant to decide which syntactic structure corresponds to such expressions: (a) right-branching analysis, (b) complex preposition analysis, and (c) layered head analysis (Figure 1).[6]

Figure 1: 
Three candidate syntactic structures.

Figure 1:

Three candidate syntactic structures.

In a right-branching analysis (Figure 1a), even though the NP the stage is part of the PP of the stage (down to the right of the tree), it is considered the complement of the head preposition in. This analysis is favored if the expression alternates with a genitive construction (e.g. on the part ofon his part). In a layered-head analysis (Figure 1c), the expression is divided into the head PP in the middle and the complement PP of the stage. This analysis is chosen if the fronting of Prep2 + relativised X is possible (he was in league with herthe woman with whom he was in league). According to Huddleston & Pullum, the complex-preposition analysis (Figure 1b) has “some initial intuitive appeal” but is blocked if one manipulation can break the string of contiguous words that an expression consists of, providing that the meaning is unchanged. Some expressions disallow all the syntactic manipulations listed above (e.g. by dint of, in lieu of, or in view of), but Huddleston & Pullum claim that this is not enough to warrant a complex-preposition analysis. They argue that these expressions belong to the same paradigm as expressions amenable to a right-branching or layered-head analysis. Their only specificity is their extreme degree of fossilization. In the same vein, Seppänen et al. (1994) posit four syntactic criteria that are specific to Prep2 and that supposedly challenge the constituent status of complex prepositions: fronting, coordination, ellipsis, and interpolation. Like Huddleston & Pullum, Seppänen et al. (1994: 11) concede that some Prep1 NP Prep2 expressions display a certain degree of syntactic “fixity” and idiomaticity. Yet, no matter how fixed and idiomatic an expression is recognized to be, the preservation of existing syntactic structure prevails in their approach as long as one manipulation is observed. Syntax is viewed as non-adaptive, radically separated from meaning, and therefore impervious to usage.

Purely formal approaches suffer from the absence of validation from usage. According to Hoffmann (2005), the constituency tests proposed by Huddleston & Pullum and Seppänen et al. are used too uncritically, and the validity of their constructed examples needs to be evaluated in the context of samples of authentic language. Hoffmann concedes that some complex prepositions do not satisfy all the constituency tests. However, because even Seppänen et al. admit that some Prep1 NP Prep2 are more fixed than others, it would seem legitimate to acknowledge this gradient, for example by assigning a score based on how many constituency tests are passed. Seppänen et al. reject such a gradient on theoretical grounds: one counter-example is enough to deprive an expression of its unit status. Using the 100M-word British National Corpus, Hoffmann finds that each of the four constituency tests proposed by Seppänen et al. yields rare structural variants. He also finds that the constituency tests admit variants that do not challenge the unit status of some Prep1 NP Prep2 expressions and that such variants are far more frequent in the corpus than those that challenge constituency. In the light of corpus data, constituency is therefore not an all-or-nothing affair.

More importantly, deep-parsing analyses like the above involve underlying levels of syntax that are unlikely to be accessible to all language users (Da̧browska 2012, 2014). In fact, speakers commonly rely on low-level generalizations to chunk expressions into exemplars that extend across artificially established constituent boundaries (Pijpops and Van de Velde 2016). This suggests that speakers commonly work with abstractions that emerge in a bottom-up fashion from the concrete examples they encounter, regardless of deep structure. In the context of complex prepositions, Beckner and Bybee (2009) show that chunking and categorization impact constituency because such domain-general processes have semantic effects and change incrementally over time. For example, the sequence in spite of is gradually forming into a unit because it is used consistently and repeatedly in contexts that suggest that its constituents are strongly associated. More precisely, even though in spite of is not affected by much phonetic change, spite behaves less and less like a prototypical noun and ceases to take determiners or modifiers as the pattern takes on new meanings (such as the expression of concession). The gradual loss of analyzability and categoriality is a consequence of the combined effects of frequency of use and semantic shift. What this means is that, over time, general syntactic blueprints may be overridden by item-specific routines due to the pressure of usage. Following the same logic, such pressures are likely to shape the internal structure of a given construction network. The IL construction is no exception, as the next sections will show.

2.2 The IL construction

Considering the large body of work on prepositions, relatively few papers have taken a constructional stance. One reason may be that the cognitive strategy that underlies complex prepositions is seemingly explicit and that their form is fully analyzable. This can be explained with reference to the origin of CxG, which emerged in reaction against theories that posit a sharp distinction between grammar rules and the lexicon on the one hand, and form and meaning on the other hand. As a result, CxG pioneers looked for non-canonical, idiomatic patterns such as let alone (Fillmore et al. 1988), the way-construction (Goldberg 1995), what’s X doing Y (Kay and Fillmore 1999), or the X-er, the Y-er (Kay and Fillmore 1999) to show that, although some phenomena occupy the middle ground in the lexicon-grammar continuum, they shed light on central linguistic aspects of language. With fast-growing evidence that full accounts and broad generalizations are obtained from the observation of constructions in usage, it is now widely accepted that “it’s constructions all the way down” (Goldberg 2003: 223) and that CxG is equipped to capture the totality of linguistic knowledge. More canonical and less idiomatic patterns have thereby received increasing attention in the CxG community despite their analyzability in terms of general syntactic mechanisms (e.g. Goldberg and van der Auwera 2012; Hilpert 2016).

Construction grammar is a family of related approaches, with commonalities and differences. In Berkeley Construction Grammar, the definition is rather straightforward. As laid out by Kay (2013), only general and maximally productive patterns count as constructions (e.g. let alone, what’s X doing Y). In Cognitive Construction Grammar, the approach that is adopted here, the definition has broadened since its initial formulation. Initially, a construction C is “a form–meaning pair <F i ,S i > such that some aspect of F i or some aspect of S i is not strictly predictable from C’s component parts or from other previously established constructions” (Goldberg 1995: 4). De facto, this excludes conventional patterns that are frequent enough to be granted a place in the inventory of constructions but that are not idiosyncratic. Such patterns are included in the revised definition (Goldberg 2006: 5).[7] Although some patterns are very general and predictable, they are not necessarily fully productive in actual usage because some functional constraints apply.

Accordingly, the Prep NP IL of NP lm pattern can be viewed as a complex symbol that pairs a form with a meaning (Figure 2). The form is partly schematic. It consists of two semi-open slots (the prepositions in or at, and the NP denoting internal location, here midst, middle, center, and heart) and the fixed preposition of. According to Langacker (2010), prepositions are conceptually complex as a result of their intermediate status in regard to the lexicon-grammar continuum. Indeed, prepositions have in common with adjectives and adverbs that they profile a non-processual relationship. What distinguishes prepositions (simple and complex) is that they have two focal participants (a trajector and a landmark) whereas adjectives and adverbs have only one, namely a trajector (Langacker 2012: 115–116). Although they are not part of the structural core of the pattern, the trajector and the landmark appear in Figure 2 because both are aspects of the frame that are cognitively relevant to speakers when they use the construction.

Figure 2: 
The IL construction.

Figure 2:

The IL construction.

The IL construction does not occur in a vacuum but is part of a higher-order constructional paradigm. The schema Prep NP lm locates a trajector vis-à-vis a landmark and is unspecified as to the type of location. As such, it hosts a wide range of prepositional constructions (inside/outside/near/in/etc. NP lm ). Inside this general schema, the IL construction is sister to a wide variety of formally similar schemas, each inviting a distinct construal. For example, it is formally close to the marginal location construction (Prep the edge/brim/outskirts/top/apex/bottom/foot of NP lm ), which expresses another kind of location.

The IL construction is formally and functionally consistent. The initial preposition (in/at) specifies a region in space within which the trajector can be found by specifying the dimension of the landmark. Typically, in profiles the interior of the landmark, which is seen as a container. The preposition at describes the landmark as a point (Tyler and Evans 2003: 179). The NP that follows profiles an internal location with respect to the landmark. In (1), (2), and (4), middle, center, and midst could be said to refer to a part that is approximately equidistant from the foregrounded edges of the landmark, namely the stage, the ring, and the river banks, respectively. Yet, as we shall see later, the kind of internal location denoted by NPsIL is not always geometric. Indeed, the conceptualization associated with the construction is more complex than it seems. This is particularly evident with heart. In (3), the heart of Norwalk specifies a region that epitomizes Norwalk but that is not necessarily in its geographical center. Finally, the preposition of mediates the location of the trajector vis-à-vis the landmark. Entities that serve as landmarks break down into three kinds (Langacker 2010: §36): (a) entities that are naturally characterized by spatial expanse such as enclosures, bounded areas, and geographic regions, (b) material objects that lend themselves to a locative construal, and (c) entities that are not normally construed as locations but that are construed as locative landmarks by virtue of figurative extension. This classification will prove relevant when we analyze the landmark collocates in Section 4.2.

3 Frequency distributions and productivity

The offline version of the 406M-word Corpus of Historical American English (Davies 2010) was used. Despite being one of the most commonly used large-scale resources in the study of the diachrony of English, several drawbacks are recognized to affect this version: inconsistent lemmas, malformed tokens, and incorrect POS tags (Alatrash et al. 2020). Another problem is the substantial variation in size between decades (from 1.2 million words in the 1810s to 29.5 million words in the 2000s). The corpus was updated in 2021 so as to address these issues. The 1810s decade was dropped and an extra decade was added (2010s). Duplicates were removed, and texts and annotations were corrected automatically. Nevertheless, the introduction of a new genre from the 1930s onwards (TV/movies) and the addition of texts to the most recent decades decreased the relative weight of the first five decades even more. Size gap remains an issue. Even though the dataset that this paper is based on was compiled prior to the 2021 update, it was not impacted significantly by the structural drawbacks of the ‘old’ corpus. The data were carefully inspected, corrected, and cleaned. Duplicates and false hits were removed manually. The issue caused by the uneven distribution of word tokens is addressed below.

Using a custom-made script for R (R Core Team 2021), all instances of the IL construction were extracted from the corpus.[8] The script looped over each of the 116,614 COHA files searching for the following sequence: in or at + definite article + NPIL (heart, center, centre, midst, or middle) + the preposition of + NPlm. Landmark NPs were retrieved using a regular expression that is flexible enough to accommodate compounds, nouns preceded by adjectives, and nouns that do not immediately follow of. Because “prepositions are indifferent as to the nature of their trajector” (Langacker 2012: 117), trajector NPs were ignored. The occurrences were manually filtered so as to exclude cases where the NP following the initial preposition does not denote internal location and where the landmark NP is not a locating element. Such unwanted occurrences were frequent with heart, which commonly denotes the organ or the seat of emotions, as in (5).

(5)
With all his singularities, there was in the heart of Uncle Tim a depth of religious sincerity
(…) (1843-fic-MayflowerSketches)

The dataset was divided into four consecutive periods: 1810s–1860s (period 1), 1870s–1910s (period 2), 1920s–1970s (period 3), and 1980s–2000s (period 4). Admittedly, these periods do not have the same number of decades. The periodization was designed to address imbalance. Period 1 consists of six decades whose token counts are significantly smaller than the others. 1860s being the first decade to feature newspaper data, it was integrated to period 1 to minimize genre imbalance. Period 4 consists of three decades that are significantly larger (tokenwise) than the previous decades. To prevent this period from being overrepresented, it was reduced to three decades. The remaining decades were divided into relatively equal-sized periods (2 and 3).

15,635 tokens of the IL construction were retrieved. Figure 3 shows variation in token frequency per decade, normalized by the corpus size of each decade. The steepest increase in usage is observed for middle cxn, followed by center cxn and heart cxn. After an initial period of increase reaching its peak around the 1860s, the usage of midst cxn drops until the end of the 2000s period, up to the point where its token frequency is close to that of heart cxn.

Figure 3: 
Relative token frequencies per decade for the four Prep NP

IL
 of NP

lm
 constructions (with locally weighted scatterplot smoothing).

Figure 3:

Relative token frequencies per decade for the four Prep NP IL of NP lm constructions (with locally weighted scatterplot smoothing).

One question to be addressed is whether these trends in usage can be translated into trends in productivity. One common candidate for the capture of realized productivity is type frequency (V), understood as the number of unique constructions found in the corpus. Here, this amounts to finding how many NPlm types a subschema can host.

Although one might be tempted to rely on relative type frequency, this is unlikely to work because types are not distributed uniformly in a corpus and, the larger the corpus, the harder innovations are to find. One workaround adopted by Perek (2018: 9–10) is to (a) drop the texts from the 1810s and 1820s, which are poorly balanced in terms of genre and consist of only about 1.2 and 6.9 million words, and (b) compile samples for each decade of the corpus by randomly selecting texts until the largest common sample size is reached (i.e. 13.1 million words in the 1830s). This kind of subsampling assumes that the type frequency of a construction is readily comparable from one decade to another for equal sub-corpus sizes. However, given that the token frequency of the construction changes, and given that type frequency scales with token frequency in a non-proportional way, one should first estimate this scaling for each decade (Feltgen 2020).

The fact remains that indexing a productivity measure on V does not discriminate between established forms and new forms, unlike another common metric, inspired by quantitative works in morphology (Baayen 1989, 1993; Baayen and Lieber 1991): V 1, i.e. the number of hapax legomena of a given morphological category. V 1 captures potential productivity. It correlates with the number of neologisms in a given category, which in turn correlates with the productivity of the rule at work. In the context of our construction, with a stable part (Prep NP IL of) and one slot to be filled (NP lm ), productivity was measured at the level of that single open slot.

Like V, V 1 scores cannot be compared directly, as types and hapax legomena become much harder to find in larger samples. They can only be compared fairly at the largest common sample size (Gaeta and Ricca 2006). To keep track of type frequency and hapax legomena developments across the COHA for the construction while avoiding the abovementioned pitfalls, V and V 1 were plotted for each subschema and each period in the form of vocabulary growth curves (VGCs) (Baayen 1992; Evert and Baroni 2007).

In Figure 4, VGCs compare V and V 1 for each period and each subschema at the largest common sample size (N heart  = 197 in period 1), denoted by the dashed vertical line in periods 2, 3, and 4. The x-axis charts the progress through the tokens of each subconstruction, while the y-axis shows how many types (thick curves) and hapax legomena (thin curves) have been accumulated. The steeper the curve, the more productive the subschema. At first, each curve rises steeply as most tokens define new types and hapax legomena. As more tokens are scanned, fewer types and hapax legomena are found, and the curve flattens slightly.

Figure 4: 
Vocabulary growth curves across the four periods (bold curves: V; thin curves: V
1).

Figure 4:

Vocabulary growth curves across the four periods (bold curves: V; thin curves: V 1).

Productivity is not a matter of how many times a construction is attested. Although middle cxn is the most frequent construction in the COHA (N middle  = 5,821), followed by midst cxn (N midst  = 4,243), center cxn (N center  = 4,224), and heart cxn (N heart  = 1,347), the two most productive subschemas are midst cxn and heart cxn, as evidenced by the topmost positions of their curves and the fact that these have the steepest slopes. The productivity levels of middle cxn and center cxn are lower. Even though midst cxn and heart cxn are the least used subschemas (token-wise), they attract the highest diversity of NPlm types. Productivity is a sign that the constructional paradigm under study is dynamic and that some division of labor is very likely to take place among its members. Whether this translates into an increase in schematicity remains to be seen.

4 Collocational shifts, schematicity, and division of labor

This section details the distributional profile of middle cxn, center cxn, heart cxn, and midst cxn by focusing on their landmark collocates. If we consider that a construction has lexical slots to be filled, one sign of schematicity can be found in the increasing number of types a construction hosts in its available slots, i.e. host-class expansion (Himmelmann 2004). Indeed, the broader the meaning of a construction, the fewer the semantic constraints at work and, in theory, the more types the slots can host. Yet, if a construction does host more types from one period to the next, this will not count as schematization unless these types convey new, more abstract meanings. The first goal of a distributional analysis is therefore to see if and how the evolution of collocational preferences indicates a shift towards more abstract semantic domains. The second goal is to reveal if a specific division of labor has emerged among the four subschemas.

4.1 Distributional semantic models

Distributional semantic models (henceforth DSMs) are computational implementations of the distributional hypothesis: words that occur in similar contexts tend to have similar meanings (Firth 1957; Harris 1954; Miller and Charles 1991). They are used to produce semantic representations of words from co-occurrence matrices, i.e. tables of co-occurring words, with target words as rows, and their neighbors as columns. Originally, a co-occurrence matrix is populated with frequency counts (how many times the target word and its neighbors co-occur) and each row is an array of such frequencies, also known as a vector. Semantic similarities are apprehended in terms of proximities and distances between word vectors. These can be assessed visually by projecting the vectors onto a Euclidean space.

One structural aspect of DSMs that makes them particularly suited to the study of semantic shifts in a DCxG perspective is that they allow the linguist to hold a given distribution profile constant and track meaning shifts at regular intervals in a historical corpus (Hamilton et al. 2016; Kulkarni et al. 2015; Sagi et al. 2011). To justify a CxG reading of English modals, Hilpert (2016) builds a semantic vector space with the collocates of the most frequent verbs that occur with may in a 50-million word sample of the COCA (Davies 2008). The data are filtered and then arranged in a matrix in which the verb types are in the columns and their collocates in the rows. The co-occurrence frequencies are weighted with positive pointwise mutual information (PPMI). The matrix is converted into a cosine distance matrix, which is then transformed into a two-dimensional semantic vector space with multidimensional scaling (MDS). Diachronic frequency information from the COHA is then projected onto the reference semantic vector space in the form of contour plots at regular intervals (1800s–1860s, 1870s–1920s, 1930s–1990s). Perek (2016, 2018 adopts a similar approach to assess the semantic shifts of the hell-construction and the way-construction.

Hilpert and Perek use count-based DSMs, i.e. models whose vectors are generated from co-occurrence counts. Such vectors are long (with as many dimensions as there are collocates) and sparse (most of their cells are zeros). This is likely to affect the quality of the vector representations, even after weighting and dimensionality reduction have been applied (Baroni et al. 2014). This paper departs from count-based models to the benefit of predictive DSMs (i.e. models generated with feedforward neural networks), in line with more recent works in DCxG (Budts and Petré 2020; Fonteyn and Manjavacas 2021).

Predictive models are inspired by neural language models (Bengio et al. 2003; Collobert et al. 2011). Instead of counting how often a collocate c occurs near a target word w, predictive models estimate the probability of finding c near w. The resulting vectors are relatively low-dimensional (from 50 to 1,000 dimensions, generally around 300) and dense (no cells with zeros).

The CBOW model of the word2vec toolkit (Mikolov et al. 2013a) predicts a word given its context. It has been shown to outperform count models on a variety of Natural Language Processing tasks such as semantic relatedness, synonymy detection, selectional preferences, and analogy (Baroni et al. 2014). Levy et al. (2015) warn that Baroni et al.’s comparison is unfair and observe that count models such as PPMI and singular value decomposition (SVD) perform equally well if fine-tuned with an ad-hoc combination of hyperparameters (context window, subsampling, deletion of rare words, negative sampling, context distribution smoothing, etc.).

Hamilton et al. (2016) show that Skip-Gram with Negative Sampling (SGNS), the alternative model of the word2vec toolkit (Mikolov et al. 2013b), outperforms PPMI and SVD in the discovery of new shifts and the visualization of changes. SGNS predicts a word’s context given the word itself, a task that is complementary to the one addressed by CBOW.[9] First, SGNS treats each instance of (w,c), i.e. a target word w and a neighboring context word c, as a positive example. Second, it obtains negative samples by randomly sampling other words in the vocabulary, thus corrupting (w,c). Third, it uses logistic regression to train a classifier on a binary prediction task (“is c likely to occur near w?”), which amounts to deciding which of the positive example and the negative example is more likely. Finally, the weights learned during the classification task are kept and used as the word vectors.

It could be argued that word2vec suffers from the ‘one vector per word’ issue (Desagulier 2019) because it produces type-based distributional representations, i.e. an aggregation of all the contexts encountered by a single word form. In the case of homonymy, this is clearly a problem because the resulting vector conflates contexts that may have nothing to do with each other: e.g. bat as the stick used in baseball versus the winged mammal. In the case of polysemy, some linguists might be wary of the quality of the resulting vectors and turn to token-based distributional representations instead. In theory, token-based DSMs such as BERT (Devlin et al. 2019) are optimal: they do provide as many representations as there are contexts, reflect semantic complexity, and detect functional-semantic change in grammatical constructions (Fonteyn 2021). In practice, however, some problems emerge. Because the base BERT model has been pre-trained on the BooksCorpus (800 M words) and English Wikipedia (2,500 M words), its vector representations are biased towards present-day English (Fonteyn 2020). Post-training BERT to increase its performance on older forms of English is possible, but only some portions of the COHA can be used for this task. It is because fine-tuning BERT on the whole corpus comes at a huge computing cost.

Another aspect that makes word2vec a method still worth considering, despite undeniable progress in the field of token-based DSMs, is that it is not considered a ’black box’. It is possible to keep track of what the algorithm does at any stage.[10] Conversely, most BERT distributions offer access to only the last two final hidden layers. When access is granted, there is still discussion as to what layer is relevant to the semanticist (Mun 2021).[11] Finally, one may consider that the aggregated approach to meaning representation offered by type-based DSMs captures the core meaning of any given word type (Erk and Padó 2010: 92). All things considered, and given the trade-off between computational cost and the semantic generalization requirement, word2vec was considered a reasonable alternative in the context of this paper given that due attention was paid to the hyperparameter setup. The consistency of the vector space in Figure 5 below suggests that a predictive type-based model such as word2vec has not said its last word.

Figure 5: 
The annotated reference semantic vector space (in blue: concrete locations; in magenta: abstract/figurative locations).

Figure 5:

The annotated reference semantic vector space (in blue: concrete locations; in magenta: abstract/figurative locations).

I used a version of SGNS that is part of Levy et al.’s hyperwords script collection for Python.[12] This version allows the user to fine-tune hyperparameters, a possibility that is not readily available in the original word2vec distributions. To train SGNS on the whole COHA (1810s–2000s), the hyperparameters were set as follows:

  1. the original context distribution is smoothed by raising all context counts to the power of 0.75;[13]

  2. context window = ±5;[14]

  3. negative samples = 15;[15]

  4. each vector has 300 dimensions;

  5. all words whose frequency is less than 5 in the COHA are discarded.

4.2 The reference semantic vector space

Based on the dataset described in Section 3, the most distinctive landmark collocates of the four subschemas across the whole COHA were selected with an association measure, namely log-likelihood ratio (LLR) (Dunning 1993). Only the landmark NPs whose LLR score was greater than 10 were kept. The word vectors of these distinctive collocates were retrieved and mapped onto a semantic vector space, as shown in Figure 5.

The rightmost part of the plot features nouns that denote spatial locations or material objects that are amenable to a locative construal. Towards the top center, in the transition zone between spatial locations and abstract location, we find institutional and symbolic locations (capital, government, kingdom). Close to this group are collective nouns (society, congregation) and nouns denoting structured, homogeneous human communities, such as enemies. The left part of the plot features nouns that are not concrete locations per se, but construed as locative landmarks through figurative extension. These nouns denote either states (lower left part of the plot) or events (upper left part). Because the collocation scores of the words that appear in Figure 5 are calculated with respect to the whole period covered by the corpus, the resulting vector space is considered a reference point over which contours can be plotted for each subschema and each period. Given the configuration of the vector space, any translation from the right to the left is considered an increase in schematicity.

4.3 Contour lines

Contour lines connect data points of the same value. Their purpose is to indicate how densely semantic areas have been populated across the four consecutive periods. Contours are generally plotted over two-dimensional topographical maps to show elevation. Here, they represent densities of association between constructions and their most distinctive landmark collocates.[16] The curves are indexed on log-transformed co-occurrence frequencies. The stronger the association between a group of words and a construction, the higher the density in the corresponding area, as indicated by an increased concentration of concentric contours.[17]

For reasons of legibility, the subschemas are examined in pairwise fashion: midst cxn and heart cxn on the one hand, and middle cxn and center cxn on the other. The pairs were determined by comparing the collocational preferences of the four subschemas in a Euclidean distance matrix. The collocational preferences were measured with periodized LLR scores. The matrix was transformed with multidimensional scaling so as to reduce the number of dimensions to two. K−means clustering was used to group the subschemas by pairs (k = 2). The above is visualized in Figure A.1. Two subschemas are close if the distributions of their landmark collocates overlap. A constant in the four plots is the proximity between middle cxn and center cxn. The proximity between midst cxn and heart cxn is less obvious (they even belong to two distinct groups in period 3) but, as we will see below, their distributions become more similar over time.

4.4 Collocational preferences of midst cxn and heart cxn

Figure 6 displays the evolution of the collocational preferences of midst cxn and heart cxn (for details, see Tables C.1 and C.2). We know from the previous section that these are the most productive subschemas. They are also the most schematic.

Figure 6: 
Distributional semantic plots of midst
cxn and heart
cxn.

Figure 6:

Distributional semantic plots of midst cxn and heart cxn.

In period 1, the frequency peaks of midst cxn are concentrated towards the left part of the vector space, where nouns denoting figurative location (especially states) cluster. Only two kinds of spatial location are found to serve as landmarks with midst cxn: wooded and marine areas. In period 2, the frequency peaks merge, and new collocate types are gained in the left part of the space. In parallel, midst cxn makes inroads into the right part of the space. This corresponds to the period when the expansion of the construction is at its maximum. In period 3, midst cxn recedes back towards abstract locations. This is the period when the construction has the fewest distinctive collocate types. In period 4, midst cxn spreads to similar areas as in period 2, but with fewer distinctive collocate types. Most of its peaks are located in the left part of the vector space. The right side of the graph is not completely deserted as the subschema co-occurs with NPs denoting marine and urban areas. Whether the host-class expansion witnessed between periods 1 and 2 translates into increased schematicity is dubious because of the direction of the shift, namely from abstract to concrete locations. Searching the EEBO corpus (Davies 2017) reveals that midst cxn co-occurs with NPslm denoting abstract locations well before the first period covered by the COHA, a sign that it has already reached a high degree of schematization by the time period 1 begins.[18]

The most striking trend concerns heart cxn, which expands towards NPs that denote more schematic locations between period 1 and period 4. In period 1, the frequency peaks of heart cxn are found around geographic and geopolitical entities. In period 2, the peaks merge and include more similar collocates, as evidenced by wider contour lines. heart cxn is also found sporadically in the left part of the plot, suggesting the start of a translation towards more abstract locations (e.g. business, matter). In period 3, heart cxn remains grounded in concrete locations around the same ‘epicenters’. It also sanctions new landmark types as it strengthens its position in the left part of the space. In particular, the density increases around conflicts and issues. In period 4, heart cxn reinforces its expansion around the peaks observed in the previous period. The largest peak density is now observed around issues (matter, case, issue, concerns).

As heart cxn undergoes increased schematization, its contour lines rarely impinge upon those of midst cxn. The distribution can be considered complementary, although this division of labor allows for occasional overlap in terms of collocational preferences. The productivity levels of midst cxn and heart cxn observed in Section 3 can be explained by the fact that, over time, these two subschemas co-occur with more semantically diverse landmark types than middle cxn and center cxn. All in all, midst cxn schematizes before period 1 and heart cxn undergoes schematization from period 2 onwards.

4.5 Collocational preferences of center cxn and middle cxn

Figure 7 focuses on middle cxn and center cxn (for details, see Tables C.3 and C.4). The occupation in the four panels is rather similar despite relative differences in terms of density in specific areas. The collocational profiles of middle cxn and center cxn are characterized by relative temporal stability as both are firmly grounded in the right part of the vector space (i.e. concrete locations) from period 1 to period 4. Most of the lower left part, which is specific to midst cxn, is left unoccupied. This indicates that the schematicity of these two subschemas is limited. The only incursions into abstract locations are found in the upper left part of the plot. Regarding center cxn, they are limited to a few landmarks whose LLR scores are not among the highest, e.g. group (periods 1–4), company, and things (period 4). middle cxn attracts abstract landmarks from period 3 onwards: conversation, sentence, phrase, war, work. These landmarks tend to invite a temporal reading. For example, Have I come in the middle of a conversation? implies that the conversation began before the interruption. Although middle cxn co-occurs punctually with abstract landmarks, its schematization is limited. Indeed, such uses are merely figurative projections of what goes on with concrete landmarks (such as river or creek), namely the interruption of an ongoing flow.

Figure 7: 
Distributional semantic plots of middle
cxn and center
cxn.

Figure 7:

Distributional semantic plots of middle cxn and center cxn.

Compared to midst cxn and heart cxn, the division of labor observed in Figure 7 is of another kind. The overlap between middle cxn and center cxn implies that the distribution of their respective landmarks is mostly parallel. Assuming that a difference in form always spells a difference in meaning (Goldberg 1995: 67), we should expect each subschema to profile distinct aspects of the same landmarks. This remains to be confirmed in the next two sections.

5 Functional shifts

Borillo (1998) analyzes French prepositions denoting internal location from the exclusive perspective of topological criteria. Gréa (2017) argues against this view on the grounds that this would imply that the NPsIL are synonyms and therefore interchangeable.

Indeed, from an exclusively spatio-physical perspective, all four subschemas match the configurations displayed in Figure 8a or Figure 8b, depending on whether the landmark is two- or three-dimensional. Gréa (2017: 101) describes this configuration in terms of a “coincidence” between the trajector and the landmark, which “translates into a superposition relation, in which the trajector is functionally separated from the landmark although located within it.” The only minor differences concern the relative position of the trajector in relation to the landmark. Middle profiles a point that is equally distant from the extremes of a bounded line or path. Center profiles a point around which a bounded entity, such as a circle, a sphere, a square, or a cube is drawn. Although they profile the central part of the landmark, midst and heart are not as specific as to the position of the trajector.

Figure 8: 
Schematic representation of a relation of coincidence between a trajector and a landmark.

Figure 8:

Schematic representation of a relation of coincidence between a trajector and a landmark.

Gréa (2017) finds that each French preposition imposes its own construal based on landmark-specific features. This section summarizes the construals associated with their English counterparts. It shows that even if a subschema has not entered the path of schematicity, it contributes a meaning of its own that is distinct from the compositional, spatial readings depicted in Figure 8.

5.1 midst cxn and heart cxn

midst cxn invites a coincidence reading when the NPlm is construed as locative, as in (6).

(6)
Howe was in all places, and in the midst of every thing […]. (1824-FIC)

However, such an exclusively spatial construal is rare in the dataset and limited to the earlier periods. Consider (7)–(9), which focus on the NPlm forest. Technically speaking, a forest is a bounded tract of land covered with trees. In most cases, midst cxn foregrounds its spatial extension. The boundaries of the landmark are perceived as out of reach by the conceptualizer. This is marked explicitly by the adjectives boundless and thickest in (7) and (8), and implicit in (9).

(7)
[…] indeed, be supposed that a youth, reared in the midst of boundless forests […] (1832-NF/ACAD)
(8)
They were in the midst of the thickest forest, […] (1914-FIC)
(9)
He came upon [the clearing] in the midst of a forest […] (2000-FIC)

Among the landmark NPs that denote concrete locations, midst cxn selects those whose vastness can be easily foregrounded, i.e. natural, urban, and social environments: river, lake, sea, ocean, desert, room, street, city, crowd(s), etc. In context, the trajector contrasts with the vastness of the NPlm. It appears isolated, estranged, overwhelmed, or threatened.

The above exemplifies what I call the estranged construal, whose structure is inherited from the three-dimensional coincidence reading (Figure 9a). The ellipse denotes the fact that the boundaries of the landmark are backgrounded, because out of reach of the trajector.

Figure 9: 
Schematic representation of the estranged (left) and intensive (right) construals.

Figure 9:

Schematic representation of the estranged (left) and intensive (right) construals.

When the landmark collocates denote a figurative location, the estranged construal may still apply. In (10), the thoughts are conceptualized as a metaphorical expanse of confusion in which the experiencer is lost.

(10)
[…] I might get a whispered distress call from a young lady whom fate, in the person of the professor, had surprised in the midst of other thoughts […]. (1917-MAG)

Intensive nouns denote entities that are associated with “intensive quantities” (van de Velde 1995: 129). These can increase or decrease and are not primarily related to space or time. This construal is commonly conveyed by midst cxn in American English. The intensive construal applies when an intrinsic feature of an NPlm is characterized by a magnitude and when the trajector profiles the apex of the scale. In Figure 9b, the landmark is denoted as a vertical axis in bold, which stands for a scale of magnitude and the circle at the apex of the curve denotes the trajector. In (11), the NPlm glory is construed as a gradual process whose maximum level of praise or renown is reached when the fall happens.

(11)
Then I had fallen in the midst of glory, Nor ever liv’d to see this foul disgrace. (1821-FIC)

Most, if not all, of the NPs in the left part of the vector space have intrinsic features that can be intensified. They are therefore compatible with an intensive construal. The left part of the vector space in Figure 6 subdivides into nouns denoting states (lower left) and nouns denoting events (upper and upper left). Among the event nouns, we observe an increased preference for landmarks that refer to festivities (preparations, dance, feast, meal, etc.). Regarding these nouns, one might hesitate between a temporal reading and an intensive reading. One does not necessarily exclude the other. In (12a), the ceremony is underway and in full swing as the bombs fall from the sky. In (12b), the same contrast is at work between the festivities, whose intensity we assume is high, and the character’s sadness.

(12)
a.
[…] the North Vietnamese had gathered to celebrate. In the midst of their celebration a downpour of incendiaries and five-hundred-pounders had fallen out of the clouds. (2000-FIC)
b.
In the midst of the festivities Louise was discovered in tears (1898-FIC).

As far as states are concerned, the list of distinctive NPlm types that enter midst cxn shortens between period 1 and period 4 and specializes in negative feelings and emotions. Given the etymology of heart, which designates the muscular organ and, by extension, the seat of feelings and emotions, we would have expected heart cxn to perform this role.[19] After period 2, the distinctive landmarks of midst cxn refer mostly to afflictions (horrors, misery, suffering, despair) and unrest (fury, turmoil, confusion). In (13), the NPlm his anguish denotes a state that affects the trajector. midst cxn sanctions an intensive construal by profiling the apex of the psychological curve.

(13)
In the midst of his anguish he still found the presence of mind to say ‘Sir’. (1973-FIC)

Some instances are indeterminate between an estranged construal and an intensive construal. In (14), the landmark can be construed as both a hostile environment surrounding the trajector and a moment when the violence reaches its peak.

(14)
In this spot my mother, who would not quit his side even in the midst of battle, was most basely massacred. (1832-FIC)

In this respect, midst cxn is very close to heart cxn which, at first glance, invites both readings. However, the data indicate that only the intensive construal is specific to heart cxn. Admittedly, in (15a), the boundaries of the landmark are perceived as maximally distant, which induces psychological pressure onto the trajector. However, this pressure is most likely conveyed by the context. In contrast, the remoteness of the landmark is presented as desirable in (15b).

(15)
a.
There is no describing the melancholy aspect of such a settlement […] in the heart of an immense forest, through which you have travelled for miles, […]. (1845-FIC)
b.
[…] it is her dream to live in the heart of the country…somewhere utterly remote […]. (2001-FIC)

Each time, the construction profiles and intensifies the inner quality of the landmark: the vastness of the forest (a forest is, by definition, vast, as opposed to a grove or a wood), and the tranquility of the countryside.

By way of confirmation, let us focus on landmarks denoting geopolitical entities and urban environments, which are among the most distinctive collocates of heart cxn as early as period 1 (e.g. city, country, nation). Such NPs often imply human activities, whose intensity can vary along a scale. In (16a), the landmark city does not convey any intensity by itself. The role of the construction here is to invite an intensive reading by locating the trajector where the landmark is the busiest. This reading becomes manifest when we compare heart cxn to center cxn, as in (16b). This time, it is the geographical center that is profiled, as opposed to its outskirts.

(16)
a.
[…] on that narrow avenue in the heart of the city is handled the enormous traffic between the twin cities […]. (1892-MAG)
b.
[…] we cross the Gran Avenida, and continue along the wide, acacia-lined Calle de Alma to the Puerta del Sol (Gate of the Sun), in the center of the city. (1931-MAG)

The intensive reading applies regardless of whether the NPlm is bounded or not. When the landmark does not inherently involve a quality, heart cxn foregrounds derivable qualities via coercion. In (3), the trajector is found in the part of the landmark that is the most representative of Norwalk’s cultural identity.

By the end of period 4, the highest collocate density for heart cxn is observed with NPsIL denoting dispute. In this context, the construction is headed by the preposition at and occurs in a copulative pattern, as exemplified in (17).

(17)
a.
At the heart of the dispute is the groundbreaking experiment at Walter Reed. (1970-MAG)
b.
Two weeks of fighting in Lebanon have not resolved the issues at the heart of the conflict. (1973-NEWS)
c.
At the heart of the case were complex accounting issues. (2002-NEWS)
d.
At the heart of the matter is a question of legality. (2002-MAG)

heart cxn profiles the trajector as a specific participant of the event, namely the cause of the dispute. Although indirect, the connection with the intensive reading is maintained as the importance of the participant in the process is rated as very high.

5.2 center cxn and middle cxn

We commonly understand a center as the point around which a shape is drawn.[20] The center is considered as equidistant from the edges of the shape. According to Gréa (2017: 104), au centre de ‘in/at the center of’ expresses more than mere geometric centrality: it conveys a relation of functional dependence between the trajector and the landmark. In American English, this relation is prevalent too with center cxn. As with heart cxn, the choice of the head preposition (in vs. at) plays a part in the construal of center cxn. With at, the trajector is the center, as in (18a). With in, as in (18b), the trajector coincides with the center of the landmark.

(18)
a.
In ancient times it was thought that the earth was at the center of the universe. (1959-NF/ACAD)
b.
Humanism puts man in the center of the universe. (1950-NF/ACAD)

Regardless of what head preposition is chosen, the landmark is functionally dependent upon its trajector: in (18), the universe cannot work without its centers, namely the earth and man. In Figure (10a), functional dependence is denoted by the double bar linking the trajector and the landmark.[21]

Figure 10: 
Schematic representation of a relation of functional dependence between a trajector and a landmark (left) and incongruous location (right).

Figure 10:

Schematic representation of a relation of functional dependence between a trajector and a landmark (left) and incongruous location (right).

Conceptually, middle cxn is close to center cxn as it profiles a point that is equally distant from the extremes of a bounded line or path.[22] In practice, this restrictive geometric reading is rare and limited to scientific writing, as in (19).

(19)
The prorenal canals lie in the middle of the line of the head […]. (1897-ACAD)

The lack of functional dependence with middle cxn has two consequences. First, the trajector and the landmark are merely juxtaposed, which results in a relation of coincidence, as illustrated in Figure 8. Second, the location of the trajector is imprecise, unexpected, or incongruous. Compare (20a) and (20b):

(20)
a.
All eyes within were focused on a couple waltzing in the center of the floor to low music. (1922-FIC)
b.
“Don’t leave it there in the middle of the floor,” she said sharply. (1872-FIC)

In (20a), the waltzing couple are given focal prominence where they are meant to be, namely a dancing floor. In contrast, middle cxn does not impose such dependence in (20b). The trajector is not supposed to coincide with the landmark, and the coincidence between the two is incongruous. In Figure 10b, the regular circle denotes the trajector in its planned location, and the circle in bold denotes the trajector in its incongruous location.

Across all four periods, density peaks are consistently observed around NPs denoting natural water bodies and streams (creek, river(s), lake, sea, ocean, waters), urban passageways (lane, intersection), and rooms. Each time, the construction activates the coincidence construal and the context specifies its nature: simple juxtaposition, imprecise location, or incongruity, as in (21a), (21b), and (21c) respectively.

(21)
a.
Some day a ferry-boat will take fire in the middle of the river when crowded with passengers. (1883-NEWS)
b.
I was further in the middle of the river, but he was still able to swim towards me […]. (2018-FIC)
c.
He prohibited the Roman pedestrian’s custom of reading newspapers in the middle of the street. (1949-MAG)

In the data, incongruous readings are common when the NPlm refers to a road or a street. If a car is parked in the middle of the street, it is because it is parked improperly. If it is parked in the center of the street, it is because there is space to park (i.e. the parking space and the street are functionally dependent).

As early as period 1, middle cxn also co-occurs distinctively with discourse units (chapter, scene(s), discourse, conversation, sentence, and tirade). When it does, it is almost exclusively in the context of an interruption, after verbs such as break, cut off, or stop, as in (22). Center is unattested with all these landmarks, except scene which, in this case, refers to an actual theater stage.

(22)
a.
Hamish stopped in the middle of his sentence, and went to him and looked down. (1955-FIC)
b.
And in the middle of all this tirade Babs stops him with an imperious gesture […] (1920-FIC)

These landmarks do not have precise centers relative to which trajectors can be located. middle cxn emphasizes that the trajector is where it should not be.

The combined expression of coincidence and imprecise location makes middle cxn compatible with the expression of vague temporal location. In (23a), middle cxn profiles a part of the landmark that is about halfway through the century. The center equivalent is possible, although unattested, but it would refer to a specific year exactly halfway through the century (1350). This level of precision is rarely needed outside of contexts involving scientific locations or measurements.

(23)
a.
He wrote in the middle of the fourteenth century (1,344–8) […]. (1972-ACAD)
b.
?He wrote in the center of the fourteenth century.

middle cxn and center cxn operate mostly at the level of concrete locations, and the division of labor between them hinges on whether there is functional dependence between the trajector and the landmark. When there is, center cxn is preferred. Otherwise, middle cxn is used.

Both middle and midst derive from Old English midd ‘middle’.[23] Accordingly, middle cxn and midst cxn invite very similar construals with landmarks denoting concrete locations, namely the expression of incongruous location:

(24)
Phil is alone out in the middle of the field. (1990-FIC)
(25)
Her mother stopped in the midst of a field and nodded. (1992-FIC)

Because midst cxn has become rare in these contexts, incongruous location is now specific to middle cxn.

Although midst cxn, heart cxn, center cxn, and middle cxn have a spatial conceptual basis, all have evolved to the point when they cannot be reduced to a geometric reading. Each subschema frames the relationship between the trajector and the landmark in ways that are hardly ever fully compositional.

6 Sketching the IL construction network

The IL construction forms a multi-level network (Pijpops et al. 2021) in which several levels of generalizations are expected to coexist (Goldberg 2009). These generalizations are deployed in two dimensions: vertical and horizontal. The paragraphs below review how the empirical findings help characterize each dimension of the IL construction network.

6.1 The vertical dimension

Figure 11 depicts the IL construction at various levels along the vertical dimension of the network. Vertical links unite the lower-level instantiations to the higher-level schemas in a bottom-up fashion. As we go up the network, the level of lexical abstraction increases. On the basis of the distinctive associations between NPsIL and landmarks, the micro level abstracts over constructs, which are of course too numerous to be represented here.[24] Although the figure represents only one item per subschema, the micro level is of course characterized by many types. Judging from the tables in Appendix C, some of these abstractions are recurrent and more entrenched than others, e.g. in the midst of the confusion, in the heart of the city, in the middle of the road, and in the center of the room. The next level up (subschematic level 2) abstracts over landmark NPs and distinguishes between preposition heads if applicable (in vs. at). The first subschematic level further abstracts over the landmark collocates and the preposition heads but keeps the distinction between NPsIL active. Finally, the IL construction abstracts over the NPsIL.

Figure 11: 
The IL construction network (only the formal component is represented).

Figure 11:

The IL construction network (only the formal component is represented).

The chain of abstractions along the vertical axis and the resulting entrenchments at the nodal levels were operationalized by means of frequencies. In Section 3, type frequencies and hapax legomena allowed us to assess the combinatorial richness of middle cxn, center cxn, heart cxn, and midst cxn across four consecutive periods in the COHA. In Section 4, collocational affinities between the subschemas and their distinctive landmark NPs were compared across the same periods as a way of tracking semantic shifts.

The above paragraphs suggest that the constructicon is elaborated in a bottom-up fashion as a series of cumulative abstractions from the most concrete (constructs) to the most abstract level (the construction). In other words, the vertical links have some upward orientation. Conversely, because a construction serves as a cognitive template for language production and comprehension, we should also consider that vertical links operate in a downwards fashion. In the context of the IL construction, the data suggest that the extension of midst cxn and heart cxn beyond concrete locations became entrenched over time via the regular and consistent pairings of their formal strata with contexts featuring landmarks that were not locative sensu stricto. Once this functional extension was sufficiently entrenched, the new meanings (e.g. the estranged and the intensive readings) became available to speakers to express new relations between trajectors and landmarks. More precisely, they were activated from the subschematic template down to the constructs. Thus, we have good reasons to think that vertical links are bidirectional.

6.2 The horizontal dimension

On top of vertical links, horizontal connections are equally relevant to fully describe the internal dynamics of a construction network (Diessel 2019). Because we set out to characterize the divisions of labor that operate among the four subschemas, we focused on those that unite the nodes at the most immediate subschematic level in the IL constructicon. After building a semantic vector space with the most distinctive landmark collocates of each pattern, curves indexed on association scores were plotted for each of the four consecutive periods. The examination of how such collocation preferences shifted over time served the double purpose of spotting cases of ongoing schematization and identifying divisions of labor, two kinds of which were found: parallel (between middle cxn and center cxn), and complementary (between midst cxn and heart cxn). It was found that the first kind involved more collocational overlap than the second kind.

Recent discussions in DCxG invite us to assess these horizontal relations in terms of competition. De Smet et al. (2018) identify two competition models: “simple” and “extended”. The first model is a legacy of functional linguistics (e.g. Bolinger 1977). It allows no functional overlap (no synonymy) and form-function mappings ideally converge towards one-to-one relations (isomorphism). Consequently, when two functionally equivalent expressions compete, one is bound to replace the other. De Smet et al. consider this model inadequate because it ignores that functional overlap is common (Van de Velde 2014), it does not explain why and how functional overlap arises, and it fails to predict two expected consequences of the isomorphic ideal at the diachronic level, namely substitution and differentiation (Petré 2014). The second model is more elaborate: the division of labour between two competing expressions is shaped by language-internal motivations, e.g. how pragmatic constraints influence syntactic realizations (MacWhinney 2014; Rohdenburg 1996). When two functionally similar expressions compete, one is better suited than the other in some contexts, and the other is better suited in other contexts. The “extended competition” model explains existing divisions of labor but falls short of predicting emerging ones, especially cases of differentiation.

De Smet et al. (2018) propose a revised model based on attraction and differentiation. Analogical attraction is the process by which, when functionally similar constructions come to overlap, they tend to become more similar over time, by virtue of their anchoring in a broader constructional network. Differentiation can be seen as the situation when divisions of labor reorganize over time. Whereas “attraction is a normal response to situations of functional overlap” (De Smet et al. 2018: 204), differentiation is considered exceptional: “[differentiation] arises as a more or less accidental by-product of the relations in a bigger constructional network.” (De Smet et al. 2018: 223). Based on a diachronic study of the dative and benefactive alternations, Traugott (2020) argues otherwise: differentiation systematically arises from attraction.

Unlike the above works, the present paper has not investigated the evolution of syntactic changes, focusing instead on collocational preferences as a proxy to detect functional shifts (Section 4). Despite this methodological difference, the trends that emerge in Figures 6 and 7 echo some aspects of the model based on the attraction-differentiation dyad. From period 2 onwards, heart cxn and midst cxn come to display increasing functional overlap: both are compatible with the intensive construal, which is sometimes hard to distinguish from the estranged construal. The leftwise shift of heart cxn is a sign that it is attracted to midst cxn, whose role as an attractor is facilitated by its high degree of entrenchment by the end of period 1. A reorganization of the constructional network is at play in periods 3 and 4, when the two subschemas seem to divide up the task of locating trajectors in abstract locations. In other words, once the analogical logic has taken hold, differentiation takes place. In this case, to paraphrase Traugott (2020), attraction and differentiation are indeed intertwined. As noted earlier, midst cxn makes inroads into the right part of the space in period 2, where middle cxn and center cxn are firmly grounded. Because this incursion is only temporary, and because it happens when midst cxn starts losing ground to middle cxn and center cxn (Figure 3), it is likely that the constructional network ultimately optimizes the expression of concrete internal location to the benefit of the latter two.

Despite local differences, middle cxn and center cxn coexist in the right part of the semantic vector space across all periods. This functional stability suggests that the constructional network sanctions both subschemas at the same level when it comes to expressing concrete internal location. Judging from the contour lines in Figure 7, one might think that the subschemas are synonyms. Admittedly, both operate in the same broad functional domain. Yet, as seen in Section 5, they do not fully alternate: center cxn is better suited to the expression of a relation of functional dependence between the trajector and the landmark, whereas middle cxn is more adapted to the expression of incongruous location. Once more, attraction and differentiation are like “two sides of the same coin” (Traugott 2020: 574).

Figure 12 focuses on the first subschematic level of Figure 11 and includes the functional poles identified in Section 5. Bold lines denote form–meaning pairings that have become central by the end of period 4. The dashed line denotes a pairing that has become secondary or obsolete. Grey arrows denote an etymological connection with the coincidence construal. Given the extent of meaning change, this reading is not distinctive of any construction anymore. Subschemas can be connected horizontally by virtue of (a) a functional pole that they share, or (b) a functional domain in common. heart cxn is connected horizontally to midst cxn by virtue of a pairing in common with the intensive construal. heart cxn and center cxn are also connected due to their common pairing with the functional-dependency reading. center cxn and middle cxn are connected horizontally via the domain in which they both operate: concrete locations.

Figure 12: 
Form–function mappings in the IL construction network.

Figure 12:

Form–function mappings in the IL construction network.

Regardless of which model applies, competition is arguably not the most explanatory concept to capture the internal dynamics at work in the IL construction network. None of them appear to have lost ground over the period under consideration. Rather than have middle cxn, center cxn, heart cxn, and midst cxn compete, the IL construction network has optimized their distribution so that all the relevant aspects of its semantic space are covered.

7 Conclusion

A DCxG approach avoids the aporias of preposition-centered debates on the constituent status of complex prepositions by focusing on usage. Collocational affinities between the prepositional patterns and their preferred landmarks show that, although analyzable in terms of general syntactic mechanisms, the four schemas subsumed by Prep NP IL of NP lm display regularities with respect to frequency of use, productivity, schematicity, and meaning.

Each subschema has evolved beyond the expression of spatial coincidence to frame the relation of internal location between the trajector and the landmark in its own way. With bounded landmarks, midst cxn tends to construe the boundaries as out of reach, which results in a situation of estrangement for the trajector. With unbounded landmarks, midst cxn intensifies properties that are often detrimental to the trajector. Diachronically, the subschema displays an increasing preference for the latter configuration. heart cxn has the most versatile distribution as its distinctive landmark collocates span the whole vector space. Most of the time, it imposes an intensive construal. Finally, center cxn and middle cxn divide up the task of locating the trajector with respect to spatial landmarks. The former imposes a relation of functional dependence whereas the latter implies that the position of the trajector is incongruous vis-à-vis the landmark.

The network offers two ways of optimizing the expression of internal location by means of four subschematic constructions. One involves the complementary occupation of a given part of the functional space, with little overlap. The other implies a peaceful coexistence in the opposite part of the same space. Unlike optimization, competition is probably not the most likely relation between these nodes at the subschematic level. In fact, competition may not be the default relation between subschematic nodes, regardless of the construction network.

Data availability statement

All primary data and code used in this study are available via the Open Science Framework, and can be retrieved from https://osf.io/x32jn/.


Corresponding author: Guillaume Desagulier, Université Paris 8, UMR 7114 MoDyCo, Institut Universitaire de France, Saint-Denis, France, E-mail:

Acknowledgments

This paper has benefited from extensive and constructive comments by two anonymous reviewers. I am grateful for their input.

Appendix A

 
Figure A.1: MDS visualization of proximities and distances between midstcxn, heartcxn, middlecxn, and centercxn.

Figure A.1: MDS visualization of proximities and distances between midstcxn, heartcxn, middlecxn, and centercxn.

Table B.1:

Top 20 landmark collocates of midst cxn in the EEBO corpus.

NPlm 1560s 1570s 1580s 1590s 1600s 1610s 1620s 1630s 1640s 1650s 1660s 1670s 1680s 1690s
enemies 1 1 1 2 12 12 11 34 34 64 28 41 57 23
sea 1 1 4 9 19 7 16 23 16 57 19 19 36 23
church 0 1 2 3 13 5 12 67 18 28 35 18 15 9
people 0 0 4 2 15 2 5 20 14 31 33 35 37 21
winter 0 1 1 1 9 12 8 22 9 42 29 22 24 32
garden 0 1 2 3 4 6 9 4 17 43 17 12 22 27
flames 0 0 0 1 1 5 3 4 15 22 35 32 20 21
city 0 0 0 2 4 6 1 2 9 24 19 31 37 14
paradise 0 1 1 1 3 5 5 7 11 25 22 15 30 21
troubles 1 2 5 1 0 7 8 10 12 20 15 11 20 18
fire 0 2 2 7 2 4 11 5 11 19 18 16 13 12
throne 0 0 0 0 2 9 2 1 8 29 14 9 30 17
house 1 1 1 3 4 9 10 10 11 26 13 12 11 5
torments 0 0 1 7 1 2 9 6 6 8 19 18 13 18
heaven 0 0 0 1 1 2 1 1 25 24 10 15 17 9
land 1 0 0 4 3 2 8 5 15 21 10 7 16 5
wolves 0 0 0 0 0 0 1 4 7 13 9 17 20 25
earth 0 0 0 2 2 6 7 7 12 25 5 3 15 11
summer 0 0 0 0 2 5 4 8 2 23 13 14 9 13
afflictions 0 3 2 1 5 2 3 4 11 27 10 4 7 11
dangers 0 0 1 0 2 4 4 4 11 17 6 9 16 16
Appendix C

Table C.1:

Top distinctive landmarks of midst cxn per period.

period 1 period 2
NPlm raw freq. coll. strength NPlm raw freq. coll. strength
crowd 8 7.31 confusion 18 28.97
grief 4 6.15 street 2 20.2
confusion 3 4.61 excitement 9 17.9
excitement 3 4.61 tumult 8 15.91
sea 3 4.61 crowd 16 15.54
storm 3 4.61 city 6 15.52
struggle 3 4.61 reflections 7 13.92
woods 3 4.61 talk 7 13.92
anguish 2 3.07 conflict 6 11.92
applause 2 3.07 laughter 6 11.92
brethren 2 3.07 performance 6 11.92
career 2 3.07 work 12 11.63
cares 2 3.07 square 2 10.65
consternation 2 3.07 struggle 8 10.55
conversation 2 3.07 campaign 5 9.93
period 3 period 4
NPlm raw freq. coll. strength NPlm raw freq. coll. strength
work 9 22.87 crisis 6 21.69
argument 4 12.81 carnage 4 18.12
campaign 3 9.6 confusion 4 18.12
excitement 3 9.6 fury 3 13.59
preparations 3 9.6 turmoil 3 13.59
uncertainties 3 9.6 campaign 4 13.34
struggle 4 8.25 recession 5 12.73
anger 2 6.39 change 3 9.31
anguish 2 6.39 society 4 9.22
bustle 2 6.39 bowels 2 9.05
company 2 6.39 crowds 2 9.05
currents 2 6.39 developments 2 9.05
debate 2 6.39 hills 2 9.05
delays 2 6.39 pain 2 9.05
dreams 2 6.39 period 2 9.05

Table C.2:

Top distinctive landmarks of heart cxn per period.

period 1 period 2
NPlm raw freq. coll. strength NPlm raw freq. coll. strength
continent 3 15.39 city 29 77.56
forest 2 10.22 mountains 8 32.07
mountain 2 10.22 business 8 25.84
nation 2 10.22 country 8 23.64
desert 3 8.97 kingdom 4 19.03
country 3 7.54 mountain 5 15.81
city 3 5.59 nation 5 15.81
wilderness 8 14.4
woods 5 13.79
earth 8 11.99
jungle 4 11.78
continent 4 10.05
matter 3 9.96
town 9 9.8
cordilleras 2 9.5
period 3 period 4
NPlm raw freq. coll. strength NPlm raw freq. coll. strength
city 12 21.33 city 30 58.99
forest 5 14.92 country 13 25.93
conflict 3 13.69 capital 6 19.23
issue 3 13.69 rose 4 18.52
quarter 3 13.69 matter 5 17.97
rose 3 13.69 case 5 15.2
wilderness 4 13.49 empire 3 13.89
business 3 9.41 issue 3 13.89
community 3 9.41 region 3 13.89
empire 2 9.12 government 4 13.73
ground 2 9.12 heart 5 9.25
matter 2 9.12 anxiety 2 9.25
mountains 2 9.12 approach 2 9.25
plant 2 9.12 concerns 2 9.25
jungle 2 5.51 mission 2 9.25

Table C.3:

Top distinctive landmarks of middle cxn per period.

period 1 period 2
NPlm raw freq. coll. strength NPlm raw freq. coll. strength
road 12 25.89 road 55 110.21
room 25 24.24 floor 67 101.91
river 6 16.02 street 31 50.91
sentence 6 16.02 room 129 44.1
street 7 13.29 sentence 24 42.33
image 4 10.64 river 10 18.51
lake 4 10.64 word 6 14.7
floor 9 8.87 kitchen 10 12.55
forenoon 2 5.3 forenoon 5 12.24
head 2 5.3 lane 5 12.24
parlor 2 5.3 sidewalk 5 12.24
word 2 5.3 bar 4 9.79
stream 4 4.22 phrase 4 9.79
channel 2 2.1 back 7 9
field 2 2.1 lake 9 8.79
period 3 period 4
NPlm raw freq. coll. strength NPlm raw freq. coll. strength
floor 46 24.65 street 80 97.11
road 25 21.43 road 79 77.96
street 20 17.09 city 9 33.78
desert 12 14.58 floor 46 26.28
lake 8 13.66 desert 21 24.07
sentence 15 12.77 ocean 15 22.05
conversation 7 11.95 living 26 17.39
sidewalk 6 10.23 highway 10 14.68
word 6 10.23 village 2 13.57
world 2 8.76 hallway 9 13.21
bridge 5 8.52 river 9 13.21
river 10 8.48 conversation 20 13
ocean 4 6.82 sidewalk 12 11.88
song 4 6.82 intersection 8 11.74
block 8 5.86 parking 8 11.74

Table C.4:

Top distinctive landmarks of center cxn per period.

period 1 period 2
NPlm raw freq. coll. strength NPlm raw freq. coll. strength
apartment 4 13.5 room 141 110.81
bookplate 3 10.1 table 30 35.63
picture 3 10.1 circle 16 19.7
room 15 8.29 stage 15 19.31
earth 2 6.72 enclosure 6 17.17
foreground 2 6.72 village 13 16.83
fort 2 6.72 square 15 16.16
front 2 6.72 road 3 15.78
mass 2 6.72 lodge 7 14.55
pit 2 6.72 clearing 7 11.59
yard 2 6.72 earth 12 9.51
group 4 5.15 hall 11 8.89
forehead 3 4.19 bottom 3 8.57
line 3 3.01 cage 3 8.57
floor 5 2.28 ground 3 8.57
period 3 period 4
NPlm raw freq. coll. strength NPlm raw freq. coll. strength
room 74 55.66 room 129 74
stage 11 11.49 street 2 43.42
head 4 10.88 road 6 27.64
house 4 10.88 table 38 23.79
world 11 10.09 universe 14 23.59
cabin 3 8.16 circle 14 15.83
compartment 3 8.16 ring 11 14.44
earth 3 8.16 picture 7 11.77
lobby 3 8.16 chest 19 11.26
semicircle 3 8.16 group 11 10.5
campus 4 6.47 square 11 10.5
city 13 6.34 light 4 9.76
circle 7 5.72 step 4 9.76
bottom 2 5.43 galaxy 6 9.61
cave 2 5.43 palm 6 9.61

References

Alatrash, Reem, Dominik Schlechtweg, Jonas Kuhn & Sabine Schulte im Walde. 2020. CCOHA: Clean corpus of historical American English. In Proceedings of the 12th Conference on Language Resources and Evaluation (LREC 2020), 6958–6966. https://acianthoiogy.org/2020.lrec-1.859.pdf.Search in Google Scholar

Baayen, Rolf Harald. 1989. A corpus-based approach to morphological productivity. Statistical analysis andpsycholinguistic interpretation. Amsterdam: Centrum Wiskunde en Informatica.Search in Google Scholar

Baayen, Rolf Harald. 1992. Quantitative aspects of morphological productivity. In Geert Booij & Jaap van Marle (eds.), Yearbook of morphology 1991, 109–149. Dordrecht & London: Kluwer.Search in Google Scholar

Baayen, Rolf Harald. 1993. On frequency, transparency and productivity. In Geert Booij & Jaap van Marle (eds.), Yearbook of morphology 1992, 181–208. Dordrecht & London: Kluwer.Search in Google Scholar

Baayen, Rolf Harald & Rochelle Lieber. 1991. Productivity and English derivation: A corpus-based study. Linguistics 29. 801–843. https://doi.org/10.1515/ling.1991.29.5.801.Search in Google Scholar

Baròdal, Jóhanna, Elena Smirnova, Lotte Sommerer & Spike Gildea (eds.). 2015. Diachronic construction grammar. Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing Company.Search in Google Scholar

Baroni, Marco, Georgiana Dinu & Germán Kruszewski. 2014. Don’t count, predict! A systematic comparison of context-counting vs. context-predicting semantic vectors. In Proceedings of the 52nd annual meeting of the Association for Computational Linguistics, vol. 1: Long papers, 238–247. Baltimore, Maryland: Association for Computational Linguistics. https://aclanthology.org/P14-1023.Search in Google Scholar

Beckner, Clay & Joan Bybee. 2009. A usage-based account of constituency and reanalysis. Language Learning 59. 27–46. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9922.2009.00534.x.Search in Google Scholar

Bengio, Yoshua, Réjean Ducharme, Pascal Vincent & Christian Janvin. 2003. A neural probabilistic language model. Journal of Machine Learning Research 3. 1137–1155.Search in Google Scholar

Biber, Douglas, Stig Johansson, Geoffrey Leech, Susan Conrad & Edward Finegan. 1999. Longman grammar of spoken and written English. London: Longman.Search in Google Scholar

Bolinger, Dwight. 1977. Meaning and form. London: Longman.Search in Google Scholar

Borillo, Andrée. 1998. L’espace et son expression en français. Paris: Editions Ophrys.Search in Google Scholar

Budts, Sara. 2020. On periphrastic do and the modal auxiliaries. A connectionist approach to language change. Antwerpen: Universiteit Antwerpen Doctoral dissertation.Search in Google Scholar

Budts, Sara & Peter Petré. 2020. Putting connections centre stage in diachronic Construction Grammar. In Lotte Sommerer & Elena Smirnova (eds.), Nodes and networks in diachronic construction grammar, vol. 27, 317–351. Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing Company.Search in Google Scholar

center, n. 2021. In OED online. Oxford University Press. https://www-oed-com.faraway.parisnanterre.fr/viewdictionaryentry/Entry/29696 (9 March 2021).Search in Google Scholar

Collobert, Ronan, Jason Weston, Léon Bottou, Michael Karlen, Koray Kavukcuoglu & Pavel Kuksa. 2011. Natural language processing (almost) from scratch. Journal of Machine Learning Research 12. 2493–2537.Search in Google Scholar

Da̧browska, Ewa. 2012. Different speakers, different grammars: Individual differences in native language attainment. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism 2(3). 219–253.Search in Google Scholar

Da̧browska, Ewa. 2014. Recycling utterances: A speaker’s guide to sentence processing. Cognitive Linguistics 25(4). 617–653.Search in Google Scholar

Davies, Mark. 2008. The Corpus of Contemporary American English (COCA). https://www.english-corpora.org/coca/.Search in Google Scholar

Davies, Mark. 2010. The Corpus of Historical American English (COHA). https://www.english-corpora.org/coha/.Search in Google Scholar

Davies, Mark. 2017. Early English Books Online (EEBO), Phase 1. https://www.engiish-corpora.org/eebo/.Search in Google Scholar

De Smet, Hendrik, Frauke D’hoedt, Lauren Fonteyn & Kristel Van Goethem. 2018. The changing functions of competing forms: Attraction and differentiation. Cognitive Linguistics 29(2). 197–234. https://doi.org/10.1515/cog-2016-0025.Search in Google Scholar

Desagulier, Guillaume. 2019. Can word vectors help corpus linguists? Studia Neophilologica 91(2). 219–240. https://doi.org/10.1080/00393274.2019.1616220.Search in Google Scholar

Devlin, Jacob, Ming-Wei Chang, Kenton Lee & Kristina Toutanova. 2019. BERT: Pre-training of deep bidirectional transformers for language understanding. In Proceedings of the 2019 conference of the north American chapter of the Association for Computational Linguistics: Human language technologies, vol. 1 (Long and Short Papers), 4171–4186. Minneapolis, Minnesota: Association for Computational Linguistics. https://aclanthology.org/N19-1423.Search in Google Scholar

Diessel, Holger. 2019. The grammar network: How linguistic structure is shaped by language use. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Search in Google Scholar

Dunning, Ted E. 1993. Accurate methods for the statistics of surprise and coincidence. Computational Linguistics 19(1). 61–74.Search in Google Scholar

Earle, John. 1873. The philology of the English tongue. Oxford: Clarendon Press.Search in Google Scholar

Erk, Katrin & Sebastian, Padó. 2010. Exemplar-based models for word meaning in context. In Proceedings of the ACL 2010 conference (short papers), 92–97.Search in Google Scholar

Evert, Stefan & Marco Baroni. 2007. zipfR: Word frequency distributions in R. In Proceedings of the 45th annual meeting of the Association for Computational Linguistics, posters and demonstrations sessions, 29–32 (R package version 0.6-70 of 2020-10-10). Prague: Czech Republic.Search in Google Scholar

Feltgen, Quentin. 2020. Diachronic emergence of Zipf-like patterns in construction-specific frequency distributions: A quantitative study of the way too construction. Lexis. Journal in English Lexicology 16.Search in Google Scholar

Fillmore, Charles, Paul Kay & Catherine O’Connor. 1988. Regularity and idiomaticity in grammatical constructions: The case of let alone. Language 64(3). 501–538. https://doi.org/10.2307/414531.Search in Google Scholar

Firth, John Rupert. 1957. A synopsis of linguistic theory 1930-55. In Studies in Linguistic Analysis (special volume of the Philological Society), vol. 1952–59, 1–32. Oxford: The Philological Society.Search in Google Scholar

Fonteyn, Lauren. 2020. What about grammar? Using BERT embeddings to explore functional-semantic shifts of semi-lexical and grammatical constructions. In Folgert Karsdorp, Barbara McGillivray, Adina Nerghens & Melvin Wevers (eds.), Proceedings of the Conference on Computational Humanities Research (CHR 2020), 257–268.Search in Google Scholar

Fonteyn, Lauren. 2021. Varying abstractions: A conceptual vs. distributional view on prepositional polysemy. Glossa 6(1). https://doi.org/10.5334/gjgl.1323.Search in Google Scholar

Fonteyn, Lauren & Enrique, Manjavacas. 2021. Adjusting scope: A computational approach to case-driven research on semantic change. In Maud Ehrmann, Folgert Karsdorp, Melvin Wevers, Tara Lee Andrews, Manuel Burghardt, Mike Kestemont, Enrique Manjavacas, Michael Piotrowski & Joris van Zundert (eds.), Proceedings of the Conference on Computational Humanities Research (CHR 2021), 280–298. Amsterdam: CEUR Workshop Proceedings. http://ceur-ws.org/Vol-2989/long_paper26.pdf.Search in Google Scholar

Gaeta, Livio & Davide Ricca. 2006. Productivity in Italian word formation: A variable-corpus approach. Linguistics 44(1). 57–89. https://doi.org/10.1515/ling.2006.003.Search in Google Scholar

Goldberg, Adele E. 1995. Constructions: A construction grammar approach to argument structure. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Search in Google Scholar

Goldberg, Adele E. 2003. Constructions: A new theoretical approach to language. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 7(5). 219–224. https://doi.org/10.1016/s1364-6613(03)00080-9.Search in Google Scholar

Goldberg, Adele E. 2006. Constructions at work: The nature of generalization in language. Oxford & New York: Oxford University Press.Search in Google Scholar

Goldberg, Adele E. 2009. The nature of generalization in language. Cognitive Linguistics 20(1). 93–127. https://doi.org/10.1515/cogl.2009.005.Search in Google Scholar

Goldberg, Adele E. 2019. Explain me this: Creativity, competition, and the partial productivity of constructions. Princeton: Princeton University Press.Search in Google Scholar

Goldberg, Adele E. & Johan van der Auwera. 2012. This is to count as a construction. Folia Linguistica 46(1). 109–132. https://doi.org/10.1515/flin.2012.4.Search in Google Scholar

Gréa, Philippe. 2017. Inside in French. Cognitive Linguistics 28(1). 77–130.Search in Google Scholar

Hamilton, William, L., Jure Leskovec & DanJurafsky. 2016. Diachronic word embeddings reveal statistical laws of semantic change. In Proceedings of the 54th annual meeting of the Association for Computational Linguistics, 1489–1501. Berlin: Association for Computational Linguistics. https://arxiv.org/pdf/1605.09096.pdf.Search in Google Scholar

Harris, Zellig S. 1954. Distributional structure. Word 10(2–3). 146–162. https://doi.org/10.1080/00437956.1954.11659520.Search in Google Scholar

heart, n., int., and adv. 2021. In OED online. Oxford University Press. https://www-oed-com.faraway.parisnanterre.fr/viewdictionaryentry/Entry/85068 (9 March 2021).Search in Google Scholar

Hilpert, Martin. 2015. From hand-carved to computer-based: Noun-participle compounding and the upward strengthening hypothesis. Cognitive Linguistics 26(1). 113–147. https://doi.org/10.1515/cog-2014-0001.Search in Google Scholar

Hilpert, Martin. 2016. Change in modal meanings. Constructions and Frames 8(1). 66–85. https://doi.org/10.1075/cf.8.1.05hil.Search in Google Scholar

Hilpert, Martin. 2021. Ten lectures on diachronic construction grammar. Leiden: Brill.Search in Google Scholar

Himmelmann, Nikolaus P. 2004. Lexicalization and grammaticization: Opposite or orthogonal? In What makes grammaticalization? A look from its fringes and its components, 21–42. Berlin: Mouton De Gruyter.Search in Google Scholar

Hoffmann, Sebastian. 2005. Grammaticalization and English complex prepositions: A corpus-based study. London: Routledge.Search in Google Scholar

Huddleston, Rodney. 1988. English grammar: An outline. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Search in Google Scholar

Huddleston, Rodney & Geoffrey K. Pullum. 2002. The Cambridge grammar of English. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Search in Google Scholar

Kay, Paul. 2013. The limits of (construction) grammar. In Thomas Hoffmann & Graeme Trousdale (eds.), The Oxford handbook of construction grammar, 32–48. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Search in Google Scholar

Kay, Paul & Charles Fillmore. 1999. Grammatical constructions and linguistic generalizations: The What’s X doing Y? Construction. Language 75(1). 1–33. https://doi.org/10.2307/417472.Search in Google Scholar

Kulkarni, Vivek, Rami Al-Rfou, Bryan Perozzi & Steven Skiena. 2015. Statistically significant detection of linguistic change. In Proceedings of the 24th international conference on world wide web, 625–635. New York: Association for Computing Machinery. https://arxiv.org/pdf/1411.3315.pdf.Search in Google Scholar

Langacker, Ronald W. 1987. Foundations of cognitive grammar, vol. 1. Stanford: Stanford University Press.Search in Google Scholar

Langacker, Ronald W. 2010. Reflections on the functional characterization of spatial prepositions. In Corela. Cognition, Représentation, Langage (HS-7). https://journals.openedition.org/corela/999.Search in Google Scholar

Langacker, Ronald W. 2012. Essentials of cognitive grammar. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Search in Google Scholar

Leitner, Gerhard. 2011. The Kolhapur corpus of Indian English - Intravarietal description and/or intervarietal comparison. In Stig Johansson & Anna-Brita Stenström (eds.), English computer corpora: Selected papers and research guide, 215–232. Berlin: Mouton De Gruyter.Search in Google Scholar

Levy, Omer & YoavGoldberg. 2014. Dependency-based word embeddings. In Proceedings of the 52nd annual meeting of the Association for Computational Linguistics, vol. 2: Short papers, 302–308. Baltimore: Association for Computational Linguistics.Search in Google Scholar

Levy, Omer, Yoav Goldberg & Ido Dagan. 2015. Improving distributional similarity with lessons learned from word embeddings. Transactions of the Association for Computational Linguistics 3. 211–225. https://doi.org/10.1162/tacl_a_00134.Search in Google Scholar

MacWhinney, Brian. 2014. Conclusions: Competition across time. In Brian MacWhinney, Andrej Malchukov & Edith Moravcsik (eds.), Competing motivations in grammar and usage, 364–386. Oxford & New York: Oxford University Press.Search in Google Scholar

Mätzner, Eduard. 1873. Englische Grammatik. Erster Theil: Die Lehre vom Worte. Berlin: Weidmannsche Buchhandlung.Search in Google Scholar

middle, n. 2021. In OED online. Oxford University Press. https://www-oed-com.faraway.parisnanterre.fr/viewdictionaryentry/Entry/118140 (9 March 2021).Search in Google Scholar

midst, n., prep & adv. 2021. In OED online. Oxford University Press. https://www-oed-com.faraway.parisnanterre.fr/viewdictionaryentry/Entry/118245 (9 March 2021).Search in Google Scholar

Mikolov, Tomas, Kai Chen, Greg Corrado & Jeffrey Dean. 2013a. Efficient estimation of word representations in vector space, 3781 CoRR abs/1301. http://arxiv.org/abs/1301.3781.Search in Google Scholar

Mikolov, Tomas, Wen-tau Yih & Geoffrey Zweig. 2013b. Linguistic regularities in continuous space word representations. In Proceedings of NAACL-HLT, 746–751. Atlanta: Association for Computational Linguistics. http://www.aclweb.org/anthology/N/N13/N13-1090.pdf.Search in Google Scholar

Miller, George A. & Walter G. Charles. 1991. Contextual correlates of semantic similarity. Language and Cognitive Processes 6(1). 1–28. https://doi.org/10.1080/01690969108406936.Search in Google Scholar

Mun, Seongmin. 2021. Polysemy resolution with word embedding models and data visualization: The case of adverbial postpositions -ey, -eyse, and -(u)lo in Korean. Nanterre: Université Paris Nanterre Doctoral dissertation.Search in Google Scholar

Perek, Florent. 2016. Using distributional semantics to study syntactic productivity in diachrony: A case study. Linguistics 54(1). 149–188. https://doi.org/10.1515/ling-2015-0043.Search in Google Scholar

Perek, Florent. 2018. Recent change in the productivity and schematicity of the way-construction: A distributional semantic analysis. Corpus Linguistics and Linguistic Theory 14(1). 65–97. https://doi.org/10.1515/cllt-2016-0014.Search in Google Scholar

Petré, Peter. 2014. Constructions and environments: Copular, passive, and related constructions in Old and Middle English. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Search in Google Scholar

Pijpops, Dirk, Dirk Speelman, Freek Van de Velde & Stefan Grondelaers. 2021. Incorporating the multi-level nature of the constructicon into hypothesis testing. Cognitive Linguistics 32(3). 487–528. https://doi.org/10.1515/cog-2020-0039.Search in Google Scholar

Pijpops, Dirk & Freek Van de Velde. 2016. Constructional contamination: How does it work and how do we measure it? Folia Linguistica 50(2). 543–581. https://doi.org/10.1515/flin-2016-0020.Search in Google Scholar

Quirk, Randolph, Sidney Greenbaum, Geoffrey Leech & Jan Svartvik. 1985. A comprehensive English grammar. London & New York: Longman.Search in Google Scholar

Quirk, Randolph & Joan Mulholland. 1964. Complex prepositions and related sequences. English Studies 45. 64–73. https://doi.org/10.1080/00138386408597188.Search in Google Scholar

R Core Team. 2021. R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing. https://www.R-project.org/.Search in Google Scholar

Rohdenburg, Günter. 1996. Cognitive complexity and increased grammatical explicitness in English. Cognitive Linguistics 7(2). 149–182. https://doi.org/10.1515/cogl.1996.7.2.149.Search in Google Scholar

Sagi, Eyal, Stefan Kaufmann & Brady Clark. 2011. Tracing semantic change with latent semantic analysis. Current Methods in Historical Semantics. 161–183. https://doi.org/10.1515/9783110252903.161.Search in Google Scholar

Seppänen, Aimo, Rhonwen Bowen & Joe Trotta. 1994. On the so-called complex prepositions. Studia Anglica Posnaniensia 29. 3–29.Search in Google Scholar

Sweet, Henry. 1892. A new English grammar – Logical and historical, vol. 1: Introduction, Phonology, and Accidence. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Search in Google Scholar

Traugott, Elizabeth Closs. 2020. The intertwining of differentiation and attraction as exemplified by the history of recipient transfer and benefactive alternations. Cognitive Linguistics 31(4). 549–578. https://doi.org/10.1515/cog-2019-0042.Search in Google Scholar

Traugott, Elizabeth Closs & Graeme Trousdale. 2013. Constructionalization and constructional changes. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Search in Google Scholar

Tyler, Andrea & Vyvyan Evans. 2003. The semantics of English prepositions: Spatial scenes, embodied meaning, and cognition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Search in Google Scholar

van de Velde, Danièle. 1995. Le spectre nominal: des noms de matières aux noms d’abstractions, vol. 32. Louvain, Paris: Peeters Publishers.Search in Google Scholar

Van de Velde, Freek. 2014. Degeneracy: The maintenance of constructional networks. In Ronny Boogaart, Timothy Colleman & Gijsbert Rutten (eds.), Extending the scope of construction grammar, 141–180. Berlin, Boston: De Gruyter Mouton.Search in Google Scholar

Received: 2021-12-11
Accepted: 2022-03-19
Published Online: 2022-04-18
Published in Print: 2022-05-25

© 2022 Guillaume Desagulier, published by De Gruyter, Berlin/Boston

This work is licensed under the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License.

Scroll Up Arrow