Abstract
Growing levels of polarization and out-group hostility have become fashionable explanations for the caustic politics of the Trump presidency. However, partisan and ideological identities cannot explain popular attraction to Trump’s anti-elite and populist rhetoric, nor can polarization and sorting account for rising levels of mass identification as political independents. In light of these discrepancies, we offer an explanation for the Trump era unrelated to traditional left-right identities and ideologies: anti-establishment orientations. We argue that much of what is interpreted as an expression of partisan and ideological extremism or polarization is actually the product of a deep-seated antagonism toward the broader political establishment. We first exhibit the individual-level correlates of anti-establishment orientations, finding that people holding strong anti-establishment views exhibit relatively high levels of anti-social personality traits and distrust of others. We then show that anti-establishment orientations are more predictive than left-right orientations of beliefs in conspiracy theories regarding COVID-19, QAnon, and voter fraud. Most importantly, we demonstrate that, while anti-establishment orientations are positively related to support for Donald Trump, they are negatively related to support for Joe Biden and both major parties. In short, the toxicity emblematic of the Trump era—support for outsider candidates, belief in conspiracy theories, corrosive rhetoric, and violence—are derivative of antipathy towards the established political order, rather than a strict adherence to partisan and ideological dogma. We conclude that Trump’s most powerful and unique impact on American electoral politics is his activation, inflammation, and manipulation of preexisting anti-establishment orientations for partisan ends.
Appendix
Question wording
Anti-establishment orientations (each item is 1 = strongly disagree, 5 = strongly agree):
Much of our lives are being controlled by plots hatched in secret places.
Even though we live in a democracy, a few people will always run things anyway.
The people who really ‘run’ the country, are not known to the voters.
Big events like wars, the current recession, and the outcomes of elections are controlled by small groups of people who are working in secret against the rest of us.
Politicians always end up agreeing when it comes to protecting their privileges.
Politicians should follow only the will of the people.
The people, not politicians, should make our most important policy decisions.
The political differences between the elite and the people are much larger than the differences among the people.
I would rather be represented by a citizen than a by a profession politician.
Elected officials talk too much and take too little action.
Established politicians who claim to defend our interests only take care of themselves.
The established elite and politicians have often betrayed the people.
Politics is a battle between good and evil.
What people call “compromise” in politics is really just selling out on one’s principles.
Primary source of news. What is your primary source for finding news? (please only select one option)
National or local TV news
Radio
Newspapers
Internet news websites (e.g. CNN.com, FoxNews.com, etc.)
Social media (e.g. Facebook, Twitter, Instagram, etc.)
Friends/relatives
Spread information online. I share information on social media about politics even though I believe it may be false.
Strongly disagree
Disagree
Neither agree, nor disagree
Agree
Strongly agree
Anomie. Please tell us how much you agree or disagree with each of the statements below (each item is 1 = strongly disagree, 5 = strongly agree; α = 0.62, M = 3.56, SD = 0.78):
The situation of the average person is getting worse.
It is hardly fair to bring a child into today’s world.
Most public officials are not interested in the average person.
Social media use. Thinking about the social media you use, how often in a typical week do you visit or use: 1) Facebook, 2) Twitter, 3) Instagram, 4) Reddit, 5) YouTube, 6) 4chan, 8kun
Not at all
Once a month or less
Several times a month
Several times a week
Everyday
Dogmatism. Please tell us how much you agree or disagree with each of the statements below (each item is 1 = strongly disagree, 5 = strongly agree; α = 0.60, M = 3.13, SD = 0.83):
On important public issues, I believe you should either be for them or against them and not take a middle course.
It is better to take a stand on an issue even if it’s wrong.
When it comes to the really important questions about religion and philosophy of life, a person must decide them, one way or the other.
Nationalism. Please tell us how much you agree or disagree with each of the statements below (each item is 1 = strongly disagree, 5 = strongly agree; α = 0.75, M = 3.05, SD = 0.93):
The United States deserves special treatment.
Not many people seem to fully understand the importance of the United States.
I will never be satisfied until the United States gets the recognition it deserves.
Machiavellianism. Please tell us how much you agree or disagree with each of the statements below (each item is 1 = strongly disagree, 5 = strongly agree):
I tend to manipulate others to get my way.
I have used deceit or lied to get my way.
I have used flattery to get my way.
I tend to exploit others towards my own end.
Narcissism. Please tell us how much you agree or disagree with each of the statements below (each item is 1 = strongly disagree, 5 = strongly agree):
I tend to want others to admire me.
I tend to want others to pay attention to me.
I tend to seek prestige or status.
I tend to expect special favors from others.
Psychopathy. Please tell us how much you agree or disagree with each of the statements below (each item is 1 = strongly disagree, 5 = strongly agree):
I tend to lack remorse.
I tend to be unconcerned with the morality of my actions.
I tend to be callous or insensitive.
I tend to be cynical.
Dark triad (combination of all sociopathy, narcissism, and Machiavellianism items; α = 0.89, M = 2.20, SD = 0.74).
Argumentation. Please tell us how much you agree or disagree with each of the statements below (each item is 1 = strongly disagree, 5 = strongly agree; α = 0.72, range = 1–5, M = 2.86, S = 0.98): Please tell us how much you agree or disagree with each of the statements below:
I like to argue online with other people.
I enjoy a good argument over a controversial issue.
I am willing to express my opinion online even if others strongly disagree with me.
Trust. Here are some ideas that some people agree with, and others disagree with. In thinking about each one, please tell us how much you agree or disagree with each of the statements below (each item is 1 = strongly disagree, 5 = strongly agree):
I trust the police
I trust my neighbors
I trust my relatives
Feeling thermometers. Please rate name listed below using the “feeling thermometer” slider bars. Ratings between 50 and 100° mean that you feel favorable toward the name. Ratings between 0 and 50° mean that you do not feel favorable toward the name.
Democratic Party
Republican Party
Donald Trump
Joe Biden
Partisan Identity. Combination of two questions about partisan attachment and strength of attachment.
Strong Democrat
Weak Democrat
Lean Democrat
Independent
Lean Republican
Weak Republican
Strong Republican
Ideological Identity. Where would you place yourself on a scale that goes from “very liberal” to “very conservative”?
Very liberal
Liberal
Slightly liberal
Moderate
Slightly conservative
Conservative
Very conservative
Interest in politics. Some people follow what’s going on in politics and current events most of the time. Others aren’t that interested. How often do you follow what’s going on in government and current events?
Never
Hardly at all
Only now and then
Some of the time
Most of the time
Sociodemographics:
Educational attainment (six-point scale, 1 = No high school degree, 5 = post-grad degree)
Religiosity (five-point scale about religious service attendance; 1 = never, 5 = every day)
Age (age in years, 18–90)
Household income (seven-point scale, 1 = $24,999 or less, 7 = 200,000 or more)
Gender (0 = male, 1 = female)
Race (Black: 0 = not Black, 1 = Black; Hispanic: 0 = not Hispanic, 1 = Hispanic)
Sample composition
Sociodemographic information about October 2020 sample, compared to 2010 U.S. census estimates.
Characteristic | October 2020 | 2010 census estimate |
---|---|---|
Age (median) | 43 | 38 |
High school degree (%) | 97 | 88 |
Some college or more (%) | 76 | 59 |
Female (%) | 51 | 51 |
Household income (median) | $25,000–$49,999 | $49,445 |
Race: | ||
White (%) | 68 | 72 |
Black (%) | 14 | 13 |
Hispanic (%) | 17 | 16 |
n | 2015 |
Factor analysis results
Exploratory factor analysis of left-right and anti-establishment items.
Factor 1 | Factor 2 | |
---|---|---|
Left-right dimension | ||
1. Partisan identities | 0.009 | 0.941 |
2. Ideological identities | 0.018 | 0.763 |
3. Feelings toward Democratic Party | −0.100 | −0.761 |
4. Feelings toward Republican Party | −0.048 | 0.780 |
Anti-establishment dimension | ||
1. Even though we live in a democracy, a few people will always run things anyway. (Con) | 0.508 | −0.046 |
2. The people who really “run” the country, are not known to the voters. (Con) | 0.547 | 0.027 |
3. Big events like wars, the recent recession, and the outcomes of elections are controlled by small groups of people who are working in secret against the rest of us. (Con) | 0.520 | 0.095 |
4. Much of our lives are being controlled by plots hatched in secret places. (Con) | 0.538 | 0.084 |
5. Politicians always end up agreeing when it comes to protecting their privileges. (Pop) | 0.548 | 0.002 |
6. Politicians should follow only the will of the people. (Pop) | 0.466 | −0.020 |
7. The people, not politicians, should make our most important policy decisions. (Pop) | 0.486 | −0.073 |
8. The political differences between the elite and the people are much larger than the differences among the people. (Pop) | 0.542 | −0.049 |
9. I would rather be represented by a citizen than a by a profession politician. (Pop) | 0.559 | 0.084 |
10. Elected officials talk too much and take too little action. (Pop) | 0.583 | −0.006 |
11. Established politicians who claim to defend our interests only take care of themselves. (Pop) | 0.709 | 0.023 |
12. The established elite and politicians have often betrayed the people. (Pop) | 0.647 | 0.001 |
13. Politics is a battle between good and evil. (Man) | 0.336 | 0.135 |
14. What people call “compromise” in politics is really just selling out on one’s principles. (Man) | 0.581 | 0.137 |
Eigenvalue | 4.511 | 2.777 |
Proportion shared variance accounted for | 0.407 | 0.250 |
Solution estimated using iterated principal axis factoring with a varimax (orthogonal) rotation. n = 1947. Bold values are those greater than 0.30 in absolute value.
Full regression results
OLS regressions of QAnon-related conspiracy beliefs on anti-establishment and left-right orientations, with controls.
Believe in QAnon | Satan worshippers | DT battle deepstate | |
---|---|---|---|
Anti-establishment | 1.261c | 2.207c | 2.175c |
(0.140) | (0.142) | (0.160) | |
Left-right | 0.780c | 0.692c | 1.993c |
(0.081) | (0.083) | (0.093) | |
Religiosity | 0.252c | 0.326c | 0.359c |
(0.076) | (0.077) | (0.086) | |
Interest in politics | −0.148 | −0.368c | −0.099 |
(0.098) | (0.100) | (0.112) | |
Education | 0.032 | −0.052 | −0.126 |
(0.085) | (0.086) | (0.097) | |
Age | −1.175c | −1.371c | −0.182 |
(0.109) | (0.110) | (0.124) | |
Female | 0.051 | 0.177c | 0.022 |
(0.048) | (0.048) | (0.054) | |
White | 0.135a | −0.072 | −0.151a |
(0.068) | (0.069) | (0.077) | |
Black | 0.163 | 0.226b | 0.168 |
(0.086) | (0.087) | (0.098) | |
Latinx | 0.024 | 0.046 | −0.186a |
(0.070) | (0.071) | (0.080) | |
Constant | 1.300c | 1.060c | 0.778c |
(0.146) | (0.148) | (0.166) | |
R2 | 0.158 | 0.256 | 0.289 |
n | 1944 | 1945 | 1946 |
OLS coefficients with standard errors in parentheses. ap < 0.05, bp < 0.01, cp < 0.001.
OLS regressions of COVID-19 conspiracy beliefs on anti-establishment and left-right orientations, with controls.
Group infected DT | DT faking COVID | DT coverup COVID | |
---|---|---|---|
Anti-establishment | 1.195c | 1.487c | 0.705c |
(0.156) | (0.171) | (0.163) | |
Left-right | 1.332c | −1.607c | −2.741c |
(0.091) | (0.099) | (0.095) | |
Religiosity | 0.425c | −0.025 | −0.082 |
(0.085) | (0.093) | (0.088) | |
Interest in politics | −0.314b | −0.515c | −0.312b |
(0.109) | (0.120) | (0.114) | |
Education | 0.069 | −0.017 | 0.195a |
(0.095) | (0.104) | (0.099) | |
Age | −1.034c | −0.702c | −0.275a |
(0.121) | (0.133) | (0.127) | |
Female | 0.047 | 0.084 | 0.107 |
(0.053) | (0.058) | (0.055) | |
White | 0.013 | −0.175a | −0.081 |
(0.075) | (0.083) | (0.079) | |
Black | 0.021 | 0.432c | 0.046 |
(0.096) | (0.105) | (0.100) | |
Latinx | −0.049 | 0.145 | 0.081 |
(0.078) | (0.086) | (0.082) | |
Constant | 1.043c | 2.987c | 4.339c |
(0.162) | (0.178) | (0.170) | |
R2 | 0.189 | 0.261 | 0.366 |
n | 1947 | 1946 | 1947 |
OLS coefficients with standard errors in parentheses. ap < 0.05, bp < 0.01, cp < 0.001.
OLS regressions of electoral fraud beliefs on anti-establishment and left-right orientations, with controls.
General voter fraud | Post office | Mail-in ballots | DT won’t leave office | DT cancel election | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Anti-establishment | 2.201c | 1.811c | 1.959c | 0.588c | 0.874c |
(0.165) | (0.157) | (0.160) | (0.159) | (0.153) | |
Left-right | 0.396c | −0.995c | 2.416c | −2.056c | −0.101 |
(0.096) | (0.091) | (0.093) | (0.092) | (0.089) | |
Religiosity | 0.128 | 0.163 | 0.333c | −0.086 | 0.486c |
(0.089) | (0.085) | (0.087) | (0.086) | (0.083) | |
Interest in politics | 0.363b | 0.188 | −0.140 | 0.021 | −0.396c |
(0.115) | (0.110) | (0.112) | (0.111) | (0.107) | |
Education | 0.075 | −0.140 | −0.108 | −0.005 | 0.284b |
(0.100) | (0.095) | (0.097) | (0.096) | (0.093) | |
Age | −0.720c | −0.377b | −1.169c | 0.159 | −1.372c |
(0.128) | (0.122) | (0.124) | (0.123) | (0.119) | |
Female | 0.239c | 0.139b | −0.043 | 0.185c | 0.023 |
(0.056) | (0.053) | (0.054) | (0.054) | (0.052) | |
White | −0.036 | −0.092 | −0.021 | −0.028 | −0.074 |
(0.080) | (0.076) | (0.077) | (0.077) | (0.074) | |
Black | 0.163 | 0.088 | 0.305b | −0.083 | 0.068 |
(0.101) | (0.097) | (0.098) | (0.098) | (0.094) | |
Latinx | 0.042 | 0.026 | 0.028 | 0.078 | 0.111 |
(0.083) | (0.079) | (0.080) | (0.080) | (0.077) | |
Constant | 1.428c | 2.461c | 1.256c | 3.756c | 1.916c |
(0.171) | (0.163) | (0.166) | (0.165) | (0.159) | |
R2 | 0.124 | 0.150 | 0.361 | 0.245 | 0.150 |
n | 1947 | 1947 | 1947 | 1947 | 1947 |
OLS coefficients with standard errors in parentheses. ap < 0.05, bp < 0.01, cp < 0.001.
OLS regressions of feelings toward parties and candidates on anti-establishment and left-right orientations, with controls.
Trump | Biden | Rep Party | Dem Party | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Anti-establishment | 10.891c | −21.120c | −12.816c | −22.759c |
(3.301) | (3.348) | (2.933) | (2.993) | |
Left-right | 106.940c | −98.462c | 96.252c | −95.922c |
(1.914) | (1.947) | (1.702) | (1.737) | |
Religiosity | 9.265c | 10.355c | 10.847c | 10.279c |
(1.784) | (1.815) | (1.587) | (1.620) | |
Interest in politics | 9.827c | 11.230c | 10.427c | 8.817c |
(2.331) | (2.368) | (2.054) | (2.096) | |
Education | 0.584 | 6.990c | 0.712 | 6.020c |
(2.001) | (2.034) | (1.777) | (1.814) | |
Age | 0.898 | 8.491b | 1.411 | 3.491 |
(2.563) | (2.606) | (2.278) | (2.325) | |
Female | −3.200b | −1.105 | −1.780 | −0.277 |
(1.122) | (1.139) | (0.996) | (1.017) | |
White | 2.450 | −2.104 | 0.018 | −2.239 |
(1.599) | (1.621) | (1.417) | (1.446) | |
Black | −2.988 | 2.472 | −1.484 | 1.160 |
(2.043) | (2.058) | (1.801) | (1.838) | |
Latinx | −4.102a | −0.195 | −2.613 | −0.720 |
(1.658) | (1.685) | (1.471) | (1.501) | |
Constant | −29.124c | 93.037c | −7.552a | 95.378c |
(3.438) | (3.498) | (3.047) | (3.110) | |
R2 | 0.678 | 0.618 | 0.678 | 0.655 |
n | 1931 | 1931 | 1947 | 1947 |
OLS coefficients with standard errors in parentheses. ap < 0.05, bp < 0.01, cp < 0.001.
References
Abramowitz, A., and J. McCoy. 2019. “United States: Racial Resentment, Negative Partisanship, and Polarization in Trump’s America.” The Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social Science 681 (1): 137–56, https://doi.org/10.1177/0002716218811309.Search in Google Scholar
Akkerman, A., C. Mudde, and A. Zaslove. 2014. “How Populist Are the People? Measuring Populist Attitudes in Voters.” Comparative Political Studies 47 (9): 1324–53, https://doi.org/10.1177/0010414013512600.Search in Google Scholar
Armaly, M. T., and A. M. Enders. 2021. “‘Why Me?’ The Role of Perceived Victimhood in American Politics.” Political Behavior, https://doi.org/10.1007/s11109-020-09662-x.Search in Google Scholar
Atkinson, M. D., and D. DeWitt. 2018. “The Politics of Disruption: Social Choice Theory and Conspiracy Theory Politics.” In Conspiracy Theories and the People Who Believe Them, edited by J. E. Uscinski, 122–34. New York: Oxford University Press.10.1093/oso/9780190844073.003.0008Search in Google Scholar
Barber, M., and J. C. Pope. 2019. “Conservatism in the Era of Trump.” Perspectives on Politics 17 (3): 719–36, https://doi.org/10.1017/S153759271900077X.Search in Google Scholar
Bartels, L. M. 2018. “Partisanship in the Trump Era.” The Journal of Politics 80 (4): 1483–94, https://doi.org/10.1086/699337.Search in Google Scholar
Benkler, Y., C. Tilton, B. Etling, H. Roberts, J. Clark, R. Faris, J. Kaiser, and C. Schmitt. 2020. Mail-in Voter Fraud: Anatomy of a Disinformation Campaign. http://wilkins.law.harvard.edu/publications/Benkler-etal-Mail-in-Voter-Fraud-Anatomy-of-a-Disinformation-Campaign.pdf (accessed February 10, 2020).Search in Google Scholar
Berlinski, N., M. Doyle, A. M. Guess, G. Levy, B. Lyons, J. M. Montgomery, B. Nyhan, and J. Reifler. 2020. Also available at http://www.dartmouth.edu/∼nyhan/voter-fraud.pdf.Search in Google Scholar
Blum, R. M., and C. S. Parker. 2019. “Trump-ing Foreign Affairs: Status Threat and Foreign Policy Preferences on the Right.” Perspectives on Politics 17 (3): 737–55, https://doi.org/10.1017/S1537592719000999.Search in Google Scholar
Busby, E. C., J. R. Gubler, and K. A. Hawkins. 2019. “Framing and Blame Attribution in Populist Rhetoric.” The Journal of Politics 81 (2): 616–30, https://doi.org/10.1086/701832.Search in Google Scholar
Campbell, A., P. Converse, W. Miller, and D. Stokes. 1960. The American Voter: Unabridged Edition. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Search in Google Scholar
Converse, P. E. 2006. “The Nature of Belief Systems in Mass Publics (1964).” Critical Review 18 (1-3): 1–74, https://doi.org/10.1080/08913810608443650.Search in Google Scholar
Edelson, J., A. Alduncin, C. Krewson, J. A. Sieja, and J. E. Uscinski. 2017. “The Effect of Conspiratorial Thinking and Motivated Reasoning on Belief in Election Fraud.” Political Research Quarterly 70 (4): 933–46.10.1177/1065912917721061Search in Google Scholar
Enders, A. 2019. “Conspiratorial Thinking and Political Constraint.” Public Opinion Quarterly 83 (3): 510–33, https://doi.org/10.1093/poq/nfz032.Search in Google Scholar
Enders, A., and J. Uscinski. 2021. “Are Misinformation, Anti-scientific Claims, and Conspiracy Theories for Political Extremists?” Group Processes & Intergroup Relations 24 (4): 583–605.10.1177/1368430220960805Search in Google Scholar
Enders, A., and S. M. Smallpage. 2018. “Polls, Plots, and Party Politics: Conspiracy Theories in Contemporary America.” In Conspiracy Theories and the People Who Believe Them, edited by J. E. Uscinski, 298–318. New York: Oxford University Press.10.1093/oso/9780190844073.003.0020Search in Google Scholar
Enders, A., and S. M. Smallpage. 2019. “Who Are Conspiracy Theorists? A Comprehensive Approach to Explaining Conspiracy Beliefs.” Social Science Quarterly 100 (6): 2017–32, https://doi.org/10.1111/ssqu.12711.Search in Google Scholar
Enders, A., S. M. Smallpage, and R. N. Lupton. 2020. “Are All ‘Birthers’ Conspiracy Theorists? On the Relationship Between Conspiratorial Thinking and Political Orientations.” British Journal of Political Science 50 (3): 849–66, https://doi.org/10.1017/S0007123417000837.Search in Google Scholar
Enders, A. M., J. E. Uscinski, C. Klofstad, and J. Stoler. 2020. “The Different Forms of COVID-19 Misinformation and Their Consequences.” The Harvard Kennedy School (HKS) Misinformation Review 1 (8): 1–21, https://doi.org/10.37016/mr-2020-48.Search in Google Scholar
Green, D. P., B. Palmquist, and E. Schickler. 2002. Partisan Hearts and Minds: Political Parties and the Social Identities of Voters. New Haven: Yale University Press.Search in Google Scholar
Hofstadter, R. 1964. The Paranoid Style in American Politics, and Other Essays. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.Search in Google Scholar
Iyengar, S., Y. Lelkes, M. Levendusky, N. Malhotra, and S. J. Westwood. 2019. “The Origins and Consequences of Affective Polarization in the United States.” Annual Review of Political Science 22 (1): 129–46, https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-polisci-051117-073034.Search in Google Scholar
Kinder, D. R., and N. P. Kalmoe. 2017. Neither Liberal Nor Conservative: Ideological Innocence in the American Public. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Search in Google Scholar
Klar, S., and Y. Krupnikov. 2016. Independent Politics: How American Disdain for Parties Leads to Political Inaction. New York: Cambridge University Press.10.1017/CBO9781316471050Search in Google Scholar
Lane, R. E. 1962. Political Ideology: Why the American Common Man Believes what He Does, Political Ideology: Why the American Common Man Believes what He Does. Oxford: Free Press of Glencoe.Search in Google Scholar
Martherus, J. L., A. G. Martinez, P. K. Piff, and A. G. Theodoridis. 2019. “Party Animals? Extreme Partisan Polarization and Dehumanization.” Political Behavior, https://doi.org/10.1007/s11109-019-09559-4.Search in Google Scholar
Mason, L. 2018. Uncivil Agreement: How Politics Became Our Identity. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.10.7208/chicago/9780226524689.001.0001Search in Google Scholar
McCauley, C., and S. Moskalenko. 2008. “Mechanisms of Political Radicalization: Pathways Toward Terrorism.” Terrorism and Political Violence 20 (3): 415–33, https://doi.org/10.1080/09546550802073367.Search in Google Scholar
Meijers, M. J., and A. Zaslove. 2020. “Measuring Populism in Political Parties: Appraisal of a New Approach.” Comparative Political Studies 54 (2): 372–407, 0010414020938081, https://doi.org/10.1177/0010414020938081.Search in Google Scholar
Merkley, E. 2020. “Anti-Intellectualism, Populism, and Motivated Resistance to Expert Consensus.” Public Opinion Quarterly, https://doi.org/10.1093/poq/nfz053.Search in Google Scholar
Müller, J-W. 2017. What is Populism? Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press.Search in Google Scholar
Oliver, E., and T. Wood. 2014. “Conspiracy Theories and the Paranoid Style (s) of Mass Opinion.” American Journal of Political Science 58 (4): 952–66, https://doi.org/10.1111/ajps.12084.Search in Google Scholar
Oliver, J. E., and W. M. Rahn. 2016. “Rise of the Trumpenvolk: Populism in the 2016 Election.” The Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social Science 667 (1): 189–206.10.1177/0002716216662639Search in Google Scholar
Pennycook, G., and D. G. Rand. 2021. “Examining False Beliefs about Voter Fraud in the Wake of the 2020 Presidential Election.” Harvard Kennedy School Misinformation Review, https://doi.org/10.37016/mr-2020-51.Search in Google Scholar
Pfiffner, J. P. 2021. “Donald Trump and the Norms of the Presidency.” Presidential Studies Quarterly 51 (1): 96–124, https://doi.org/10.1111/psq.12698.Search in Google Scholar
Reny, T. T, L. Collingwood, and A. A. Valenzuela. 2019. “Vote Switching in the 2016 Election: How Racial and Immigration Attitudes, Not Economics, Explain Shifts in White Voting.” Public Opinion Quarterly 83 (1): 91–113, https://doi.org/10.1093/poq/nfz011.Search in Google Scholar
Rieder, R. 2020. Trump and the ‘New Hoax’. Factcheck.org. (accessed March 21, 2020).Search in Google Scholar
Rudolph, T. 2021. “Populist Anger, Donald Trump, and the 2016 Election.” Journal of Elections, Public Opinion, and Parties 31 (1): 33–58, https://doi.org/10.1080/17457289.2019.1582532.Search in Google Scholar
Smallpage, S. M., A. M. Enders, and U. Joseph. 2017. “The Partisan Contours of Conspiracy Theory Beliefs.” Research & Politics 4 (4): 2053168017746554, https://doi.org/10.1177/2053168017746554.Search in Google Scholar
Spruyt, B., G. Keppens, and F. Van Droogenbroeck. 2016. “Who Supports Populism and What Attracts People to It?” Political Research Quarterly 69 (2): 335–46, https://doi.org/10.1177/1065912916639138.Search in Google Scholar
Stecula, D. A., and M. Pickup. 2021. “How Populism and Conservative Media Fuel Conspiracy Beliefs about COVID-19 and what it Means for COVID-19 Behaviors.” Research & Politics 8 (1): 2053168021993979, https://doi.org/10.1177/2053168021993979.Search in Google Scholar
Stroud, N. J. 2008. “Media Use and Political Predispositions: Revisiting the Concept of Selective Exposure.” Political Behavior 30 (3): 341–66, https://doi.org/10.1007/s11109-007-9050-9.Search in Google Scholar
Uscinski, J., and J. M. Parent. 2014. American Conspiracy Theories. New York: Oxford University Press.10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199351800.001.0001Search in Google Scholar
Uscinski, J., A. M. Enders, W. Stefan, C. Klofstad, M. Seelig, J. Funchion, M. Murthi, K. Premaratne, and C. Everett. 2020. “Why Do People Believe COVID-19 Conspiracy Theories?” The Harvard Kennedy School (HKS) Misinformation Review 1: 1–12, https://doi.org/10.37016/mr-2020-015.Search in Google Scholar
Uscinski, J., C. Klofstad, and M. Atkinson. 2016. “Why Do People Believe in Conspiracy Theories? The Role of Informational Cues and Predispositions.” Political Research Quarterly 69 (1): 57–71.10.2139/ssrn.2268782Search in Google Scholar
Uscinski, J., E. Adam, M. Seelig, C. Klofstad, J. Funchion, C. Everett, S. Wuchty, K. Premaratne, and M. Murthi. 2021. “American Politics in Two Dimensions: Partisan and Ideological Identities versus Anti-Establishment Orientations.” American Journal of Political Science. Also available at https://www.joeuscinski.com/uploads/7/1/9/5/71957435/two_dimensions_uscinski_et_al.pdf.10.1111/ajps.12616Search in Google Scholar
van Prooijen, J-W., B. Rutjens, and M. Brandt. 2018. “Populism as Political Mentality Underlying Conspiracy Theories.” In Belief Systems and the Perception of Reality, 79–96. London: Routledge.10.4324/9781315114903-6Search in Google Scholar
Wang, A. 2016. ‘Post-truth’ Named 2016 Word of the Year by Oxford Dictionaries. (accessed May 24, 2019).Search in Google Scholar
© 2021 Walter de Gruyter GmbH, Berlin/Boston