Abstract
If one takes the view that utterance meaning is the product of interactional processes, then intended perlocutionary effects should be receiving more attention; indeed, their occurrence (or nonoccurrence, for that matter) has a direct influence on the circumstances that determine the range of possible language choices for both speaker and hearer. [1] Building on this assumption, this article is concerned with the process whereby the communication of certainty can lead to the intended perlocutionary effect. Using Damasio’s convergence-divergence zone framework, which presupposes an adaptive perspective, I argue that such effects are more likely to come about if certainty is communicated without evidential markers than when such marking is employed. The presumed effectiveness of this mode of communication would be due to the automatic nature of the mechanism involved, which mechanism relies on an interpretive process that may be seen as a simulation of recall.
About the author
Thanh Nyan is a Senior Lecturer in the Department of English and Linguistics at the University of Manchester. She has published extensively on discourse markers in French. In the past few years, she has been investigating context construction from an adaptive perspective, drawing on assumptions and findings arising from Edelman’s theory of neuronal group selection and Damasio’s convergence-divergence zone framework. She is currently working on a book on context construction.
Acknowledgments
I am grateful to my anonymous reviewers for their helpful comments.
References
Aikhenvald, Alexandra. 2004. Evidentiality. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Search in Google Scholar
Allport, Alan. 1987. Selection for action: Some behavioural and neurophysiological consideration of attention and action. In Herbert Heur & Andries F. Sanders (eds.), Perspective on perception and action, 393–419. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Search in Google Scholar
Anscombre, Jean-Claude (ed.). 1995. Théorie des topoï. Paris: Editions Kimé.Search in Google Scholar
Barsalou, Lawrence W. 2008. Grounded cognition. Annual Review of Psychology 59. 617–645.10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093639Search in Google Scholar
Cummins, Robert. 1989. Functional analysis. In Baruch A. Brody & Richard E. Grandy (eds.), Readings in the philosophy of science, 2nd edn., 495–512. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.Search in Google Scholar
Damasio, Antonio R. 1989a. Time-locked multiregional retroactivation: A systems-level proposal for the neural substrates of recall and recognition. Cognition 33. 25–62.10.1016/0010-0277(89)90005-XSearch in Google Scholar
Damasio, Antonio R. 1989b. Concepts in the brain. Mind and Language 4. 24–2810.1111/j.1468-0017.1989.tb00236.xSearch in Google Scholar
Damasio, Antonio R. 1994. Descartes’ error: Emotion, reason, and the human brain. New York: Grosset/Putnam.Search in Google Scholar
Damasio, Antonio R. 2010. Self comes to mind. New York: Pantheon.Search in Google Scholar
Damasio, Antonio R. & Hanna Damasio. 1992. Brain and language. Scientific American 267(3). 89–95.10.1038/scientificamerican0992-88Search in Google Scholar
Damasio, Hanna, Thomas J. Grabowski, Daniel Tranel, Richard D. Hichwa & Antonio Damasio. 1996. A neural basis for lexical retrieval. Nature 380. 499–505.10.1038/380499a0Search in Google Scholar
Damasio, Hanna, Daniel Tranel, Thomas Grabowski, Ralph Adolphs & Antonio Damasio. 2004. Neural systems behind word and concept retrieval. Cognition 92. 179–229.10.1016/j.cognition.2002.07.001Search in Google Scholar
Du Bois, John W. 1986. Self-evidence and ritual speech. In Wallace Chafe & Johanna Nichols (eds.), Evidentiality: The linguistic coding of epistemology, 313–336. Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Search in Google Scholar
Duranti, Alessandro. 1993. Intentions, self, and responsibility: An essay in Samoan ethnopragmatics. In Jane H. Hill & Judith T. Irvine (eds.), Responsibility and evidence in oral discourse, 24–47. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Search in Google Scholar
Edelman, Gerald M. 1989. The remembered present. New York: Basic Books.Search in Google Scholar
Edelman, Gerald M. 1992. Bright air, brilliant fire. New York: Basic Books.Search in Google Scholar
Fitneva, Stanka A. 2001. Epistemological marking and reliability judgement: Evidence from Bulgarian. Journal of Pragmatics 33. 401–420.10.1016/S0378-2166(00)00010-2Search in Google Scholar
Frajzyngier, Zygmunt. 1985. Truth and the indicative sentence. Studies in Language 9. 243–254.10.1075/sl.9.2.05fraSearch in Google Scholar
Futuyma, Douglas J. 1998. Evolutionary biology, 3rd edn. Sunderland, MA: Sinaer Associates.Search in Google Scholar
Givón, Talmy. 2005. Context as other minds: The pragmatics of sociality and cognition and communication. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.10.1075/z.130Search in Google Scholar
Hardman, Martha J. 1986. Data-source marking in the Jaqi languages. In Wallace Chafe & Johanna Nichols (eds.), Evidentiality: The linguistic coding of epistemology, 113–135. Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Search in Google Scholar
Hill, Jane H. & Judith T. Irvine (eds.). 1993. Responsibility and evidence in oral discourse. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Search in Google Scholar
Hurley, Susan. 2001. Perception and action: An alternative view. Synthese 129. 3–40.10.1023/A:1012643006930Search in Google Scholar
Koch, Christof. 2004. The quest for consciousness: A neurobiological approach. Englewood, CO: Roberts and Company.Search in Google Scholar
Kosslyn, Stephen & Olivier Koenig. 1995. Wet mind. New York: Free Press.Search in Google Scholar
Lazard, Gilbert. 2001. On the grammaticalization of evidentiality. Journal of Pragmatics 33. 359–367.10.1016/S0378-2166(00)00008-4Search in Google Scholar
Mayr, Ernst. 1983. How to carry out the adaptationist program. American Naturalist 121(3). 324–334.10.1086/284064Search in Google Scholar
Mayr, Ernst. 1989. Some thoughts on the history of the evolutionary synthesis. In Ernst Mayr & William B. Provine (eds.), The evolutionary synthesis, 1–45. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Search in Google Scholar
Meyer, Kaspar & Antonio Damasio. 2009. Convergence and divergence in a neural architecture for recognition and memory. Trends in Neurosciences 32(7). 376–382.10.1016/j.tins.2009.04.002Search in Google Scholar
Nyan, Thanh. 2007. Appropriateness: An adaptive view. In Anita Fetzer (ed.), Context and appropriateness: Micro meets macro, 72–112. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.10.1075/pbns.162.06nyaSearch in Google Scholar
Nyan, Thanh. 2011. Context: An adaptive perspective. In Anita Fetzer & Etsuko Oishi (eds.), Context and contexts: Parts meet whole, 205–233. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.10.1075/pbns.209.13nyaSearch in Google Scholar
Tomasello, Michael, Malinda Carpenter, Josep Call, Tanya Behne & Henrike Moll. 2005. Understanding and sharing intentions: The origins of cultural cognition. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 28. 675–735.10.1017/S0140525X05000129Search in Google Scholar
Tranel, Daniel & Antonio R. Damasio. 1999. The neurobiology of knowledge retrieval. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 22(2). 303–333.10.1017/S0140525X99461825Search in Google Scholar
Tranel, Daniel, Ralph Adolphs, Hanna Damasio & Antonio Damasio. 2001. A neural basis for the retrieval of words for actions. Cognitive Neuropsychology 18(7). 655–670.10.1080/02643290126377Search in Google Scholar
Verschueren, Jef. 1999. Understanding pragmatics. London: Arnold.Search in Google Scholar
©2015 by De Gruyter Mouton