Accessible Requires Authentication Published by De Gruyter August 20, 2014

A Comparison of Movement Imagery Ability Self-Report and Imagery Use in a Motor Task: A Preliminary Investigation

Carl P. Gabbard and Jihye Lee

Abstract

The present study used the Movement Imagery Questionnaire (MIQ-3) to determine the relationship between self-report movement imagery ability and performance on a motor task requiring use of visual and kinesthetic imagery. Young adults were asked to view a number sequence of 4- and 5 digits, maintain the information over a 6 s delay (blank screen) using visual imagery maintenance, and complete the sequence by finger-tapping complementary numbers using motor (kinesthetic) imagery and actually executing movements. We predicted higher movement imagery ability scores would be associated with faster movement times, and imagery ability would be significantly related to the motor tasks. Correlation results indicated no significant differences between self-report and finger-tapping scores, suggesting that in the context of the motor tasks used here, performance was independent of movement imagery ability. Discussion of this preliminary study focuses on the role of visual working memory in the motor task used here and its lack of assessment using the MIQ-3 and other current self-reports.

References

Abma, C. L., Fry, M. D., Li, Y., & Relyea, G. (2002). Differences in imagery content and imagery ability between high and low confident track and field athletes. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 14, 6775. Search in Google Scholar

Anquetil, T., & Jeannerod, M. (2007). Simulated actions in the first and in the third person perspectives share common representations. Brain research, 1130, 125129. Search in Google Scholar

Baddeley, A. D., & Andrade, J. (2000). Working memory and the vividness of imagery. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 129, 126145. Search in Google Scholar

Bartolomeo, P. (2008). The neural correlates of visual mental imagery: An ongoing debate. Cortex, 44(2), 107108. Search in Google Scholar

Bergen, B., & Wheeler, K. (2010). Grammatical aspect and mental simulation. Brain and Language, 112(3), 150158. Search in Google Scholar

Burianová, H., Marstaller, L., Sowman, P., Tesan, G., Richl, A. N., Williams, M., … Johnson, B. W. (2013). Multimodal functional imaging of motor imagery using a novel paradigm. NeuroImage, 71, 5058. Search in Google Scholar

Caçola, P., Roberson, J., & Gabbard, C. (2013). Aging in movement representations for sequential finger movements: A comparison between young, middle-aged, and older adults. Brain and Cognition, 82(1), 15. Search in Google Scholar

Collet, C. A., Guillot, A., Lebon, F., Macintyre, T., & Moran, A. (2011). Measuring motor imagery using psychometric behavioral, and psychophysiological tools. Exercise and Sport Sciences Review, 39(2), 8592. Search in Google Scholar

Crémers, J., Dessoullières, A., & Garraux, G. (2012). Hemispheric specialization during mental imagery of brisk walking. Human Brain Mapping, 33(4), 873882. Search in Google Scholar

Cumming, J., & Williams, S. E. (2012). The role of imagery in performance. In S.Murphy (Ed.), Handbook of sport and performance psychology (pp. 213232). New York, NY: Oxford University Press. Search in Google Scholar

Fourkas, A. D., Avenanti, A., Urgesi, C., & Aglioti, S. M. (2006). Corticospinal facilitation during first and third person imagery. Experimental Brain Research, 168, 143151. Search in Google Scholar

Gabbard, C., Caҫola, P., & Bobbio, T. (2011). Examining age-related movement representations for sequential (fine-Motor) finger movements. Brain and Cognition, 77, 459463. Search in Google Scholar

Gabbard, C., Lee, J., & Caçola, P. (2013). Role of working memory in transformation of visual and motor simulation. Cognitive Neuroscience. doi/full/10.1080/17588928.2013.820180#.UfZ6T1N1vx41.37 Search in Google Scholar

Ganis, G., Thompson, W. L., & Kosslyn, S. M. (2004). Brain areas underlying visual mental imagery and visual perception: An fMRI study. Cognitive Brain Research, 20, 226241. Search in Google Scholar

Grèzes, J., & Decety, J. (2001). Functional anatomy of execution, mental simulation, observation, and verb generation of actions: A meta-analysis. Human Brain Mapping, 12(1), 119. Search in Google Scholar

Grealy, M., & Shearer, G. (2007). Timing processes in motor imagery. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 20, 867892. Search in Google Scholar

Guillot, A., Desliens, S., Rouyer, C., & Rogowski, I. (2013). Motor imagery and tennis serve performance: The external focus efficacy. Journal of Sports Science & Medicine, 12(2), 332338. Search in Google Scholar

Hall, C. R., & Martin, K. A. (1997). Measuring movement imagery abilities: A revision of the Movement Imagery Questionnaire. Journal of Mental Imagery, 21, 143154. Search in Google Scholar

Hamamé, C. M., Vidal, J. R., Ossandon, T., Jerbi, K., Dalal, S. S., Minotti, L., et al. (2012). Reading the mind’s eye: Online detection of visuo-spatial working memory and visual imagery in the inferior temporal lobe. NeuroImage, 59(1), 872879. Search in Google Scholar

Heremans, E., Feys, P., Nieuwboer, A., Vercruysse, S., Vandenberghe, W., Sharma, N., et al. (2011). Motor imagery ability in patients with early- and mid-stage Parkinson disease. Neurorehabilitation and Neural Repair, 25, 168177. Search in Google Scholar

Hyde, C., Wilmut, K., Fuelscher, I., & Williams, J. (2013). Does implicit motor imagery ability predict reaching correction efficiency? A test of recent models of human motor control. Journal of Motor Behavior, 45(3), 259269. Search in Google Scholar

Johnson, S., Corballis, P., & Gazzaniga, M. (2001). Within grasp but out of reach: Evidence for a double dissociation between imagined hand and arm movements in the left cerebral hemisphere. Neuropsychologia, 39, 3650. Search in Google Scholar

Kosslyn, S. M., Ganis, G., & Thompson, W. L. (2001). Neural foundations of imagery. Nature reviews. Neuroscience, 2, 635642. Search in Google Scholar

Kunz, B. R., Creem-Regehr, S. H., & Thompson, W. B. (2009). Evidence for motor simulation in imagined locomotion. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 35, 14581471. Search in Google Scholar

Lacourse, M. G., Orr, E. L. R., Cramer, S. C., & Cohen, M. J. (2005). Brain activation during execution and motor imagery of novel and skilled sequential hand movements. NeuroImage, 27, 505519. Search in Google Scholar

Lequerica, A., Rapport, L., Axelrod, B. N., Telmet, K., & Whitman, R. D. (2002). Subjective and objective assessment methods of mental imagery control: Construct validation of self-report measures. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 24, 11031116. Search in Google Scholar

Lorey, B., Pilgramm, S., Walter, B., Stark, R., Munzert, J., & Zentgraf, K. (2010). Your mind’s hand: Motor imagery of pointing movements with different accuracy. NeuroImage, 49, 32393247. Search in Google Scholar

Malouin, F., & Richards, C. L. (2010). Mental practice for relearning locomotor skills. Physical Therapy, 90, 240251. Search in Google Scholar

Maruff, P., Wilson, P., Trebilcock, M., & Currie, J. (1999). Abnormalities of imagined motor sequences in children with developmental coordination disorder. Neuropsychologia, 37, 13171324. Search in Google Scholar

Monsma, E. V., Short, S. E., Hall, C. R., Gregg, M., & Sullivan, P. (2009). Psychometric properties of the revised Movement Imagery Questionnaire (MIQ-R). Journal of Imagery Research in Sport and Physical Activity, 4, 112. Search in Google Scholar

Moran, A., Guillot, A., MacIntyre, T., & Collet, C. (2012). Re-imagining motor imagery: Building bridges between cognitive neuroscience and sport psychology. British Journal of Psychology, 103(2), 224247. Search in Google Scholar

Moulton, S. T., & Kosslyn, S. M. (2009). Imagining predictions: Mental imagery as mental emulation. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, 364, 12731280. Search in Google Scholar

Osstra, K. M., Vereecke, A., Jones, K., Vanderstraeten, G., & Vingerhoets, G. (2012). Motor imagery ability in patients with traumatic brain injury. Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, 93, 828833. Search in Google Scholar

Pelgrims, B., Andres, M., & Olivier, E. (2009). Double dissociation between motor and visual imagery in the posterior parietal cortex. Cerebral Cortex, 19(10), 22982307. Search in Google Scholar

Sabate, M., Gonzalez, B., & Rodriguez, M. (2004). Brain lateralization of motor imagery: Motor planning asymmetry as a cause of movement lateralization. Neuropsychologia, 42, 10411049. Search in Google Scholar

Saimpont, A., Malouin, F., Tousignant, B., & Jackson, P. (2013). Motor imagery and aging. Journal of Motor Behavior, 45, 2128. Search in Google Scholar

Schott, N. (2012). Age-related differences in motor imagery: Working memory as a mediator. Experimental Aging Research, 38(5), 559583. Search in Google Scholar

Sirigu, A., & Duhamel, J. R. (2001). Motor and visual imagery as two complementary but neurally dissociable mental process. Journal of Cognition Neuroscience, 13, 910919. Search in Google Scholar

Stevens, J. A. (2005). Interference effects demonstrate distinct roles for visual and motor imagery during the mental representation of human action. Cognition, 95, 329350. Search in Google Scholar

Thompson, W. L., Slotnick, S. D., Burrage, M. S., & Kosslyn, S. M. (2009). Two forms of spatial imagery: Neuroimaging evidence. Psychological Science, 20, 12451253. Search in Google Scholar

Williams, S. E., Cumming, J., Ntoumanis, N., Nordin-Bates, S. M., Ramsey, R. & Hall, C. R. (2012). Further validation and development of the movement imagery questionnaire. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 34, 621646. Search in Google Scholar

Published Online: 2014-8-20
Published in Print: 2014-1-1

©2014 by De Gruyter