Abstract
Some Japanese predicates allow their object to be marked either with the accusative particle o, the canonical marker of an object, or with the nominative particle ga, the canonical marker of a subject. This work reports and discusses the results of corpus-based surveys on the patterns and recent trends in nominative-marking on objects. It will be demonstrated: (i) that there has been an increase in the proportion of accusative-marking over nominative-marking in recent times, (ii) that accusative-marking is more likely to be chosen in certain types of subordinate clauses than in matrix clauses, and (iii) that at least with some types of predicates, accusative-marking on the object is more likely when its referent is animate rather than inanimate. It will be argued that the effects of clause type and animacy have to do with the functional motivation to mitigate processing load and the risk of misinterpretation incurred by nominative-marking on objects.
Acknowledgments
Part of this research was presented at the 26th Japanese/Korean Linguistics conference held at UCLA in 2018. We would like to thank Yoshiki Ogawa and the two anonymous reviewers of Journal of Japanese Linguistics for their valuable comments. We would also like to acknowledge that this research greatly benefited from the NINJAL collaborative research project “Development of and Linguistic Research with a Parsed Corpus of Japanese”.
-
Research funding: This work was supported by JSPS KAKENHI (http://dx.doi.org/10.13039/501100001691) Grant Number JP15J00283.
Appendix: The lack of dependence between the effects of the factors considered
We reported some statistical findings on (i) how the birth year correlates with the proportions of ga- and o-marking on the object in different types of NA/CN-predicates, (ii) how the clause type correlates with the proportions of ga- and o-marking on the object in different types of NA/CN-predicates, and (iii) how the animacy of the referent of the object correlates with the proportions of ga- and o-marking on the object in different types of NA/CN-predicates. The findings concerning (i) and (ii) are based on two sets of data, one from BCCWJ and the other from CSJ; those concerning (iii) are based on the BCCWJ data only (the CSJ data did not suit our purpose due to their relatively small size).
In purpose to confirm that the effects of the relevant factors are independent from each other, we constructed two logistic regression models, one based on the BCCWJ data and the other based on the CSJ data. The BCCWJ-based model was constructed with four factors: (i) birth year, (ii) predicate type (derived potential, phrasal potential, desiderative, and simple CN-predicate), (iii) clause type (main clause, complement clause, relative clause, temporal/conditional clause, reason clause, and the remainder), and (iv) animacy of the referent of the object (animate and inanimate). The CSJ-based model was built with the same set of factors but animacy of the referent of the object.
It was confirmed that, in the BCCWJ-based model, the effects of the four factors are not dependent on one another and that, in the CSJ-based model, the effects of the three factors are not dependent on one another (Tables A and B).
Results from the BCCWJ-based logistic regression model.
Estimate | Standard error | z | p | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Intercept | 10.77 | 3.69 | 2.92 | <0.01 |
Birth year | −0.006 | 0.002 | −3.07 | <0.01 |
Predicate type | ||||
Desideratives | −3.15 | 0.14 | −23.11 | <0.001 |
Simple CN-predicates | 2.65 | 0.13 | 19.62 | <0.001 |
Phrasal potentials | −1.45 | 0.11 | −12.73 | <0.001 |
Clause type | ||||
Main | 0.77 | 0.20 | 3.86 | <0.001 |
Remainder | −0.08 | 0.20 | −0.38 | 0.71 |
Reason | 0.72 | 0.32 | 2.26 | 0.02 |
Rel. | −0.65 | 0.24 | −2.69 | 0.07 |
T/C | 0.17 | 0.28 | 0.63 | 0.53 |
Animacy | ||||
Inanimate | 1.30 | 0.18 | 7.11 | <0.001 |
Results from the CSJ-based logistic regression model.
Estimate | Standard error | z | p | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Intercept | 15.77 | 8.53 | 1.85 | 0.07 |
Birth year | −0.01 | 0.004 | −2.21 | <0.05 |
Predicate type | ||||
Derived potentials | 3.92 | 0.21 | 18.44 | <0.001 |
Simple CN-predicates | 6.42 | 0.25 | 26.02 | <0.001 |
Phrasal potentials | 3.08 | 0.21 | 14.44 | <0.001 |
Clause type | ||||
Main | 0.98 | 0.21 | 4.60 | <0.001 |
Remainder | 0.19 | 0.18 | 1.04 | 0.30 |
Reason | 0.57 | 0.35 | 1.65 | 0.10 |
Rel. | −0.09 | 0.28 | −0.33 | 0.74 |
T/C | 0.48 | 0.39 | 1.25 | 0.21 |
References
Aissen, Judith. 2003. Differential object marking: Iconicity vs. economy. Natural Language and Linguistic Studies 21. 435–483. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1024109008573.10.1023/A:1024109008573Search in Google Scholar
Caluianu, Daniela. 2009. Transitive adjectives in Japanese. In Jóhanna Barðdal & Shobhana L. Chelliah (eds.), The role of semantic, pragmatic, and discourse factors in the development of case, 225–257. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.10.1075/slcs.108.13calSearch in Google Scholar
Cukor-Avila, Patricia & Guy Bailey. 2013. Real time and apparent time. In J. K. Chambers & Natalie Schilling-Estes (eds.), The handbook of language variation and change, 237–262. Oxford: Wiley-Blackwell.10.1002/9781118335598.ch11Search in Google Scholar
Frellesvig, Bjarke. 2010. A history of the Japanese language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.10.1017/CBO9780511778322Search in Google Scholar
Fry, John. 2003. Ellipsis and wa-marking in Japanese conversation. New York: Routledge.10.4324/9780203484036Search in Google Scholar
Harada, Shin-ichi. 1971. Ga-no conversion and idiolectal variations in Japanese. Gengo Kenkyu 60. 25–38. https://doi.org/10.11435/gengo1939.1971.60_25.Search in Google Scholar
Hirata, Yu. 2002. Development of semantic theme markers for desiderative predicates in Japanese. In Patricia Clancy (ed.), Japanese/Korean Linguistics 11, 135–148. Stanford: CSLI Publications.Search in Google Scholar
Hopper, Paul J. & Sandra Thompson. 1980. Transitivity and grammar and discourse. Language 56(2). 251–299. https://doi.org/10.1353/lan.1980.0017.Search in Google Scholar
Kuno, Susumu. 1973. Nihon bunpō kenkyū [Studies of the Japanese grammar]. Tokyo: Taishukan.Search in Google Scholar
Kuznetsova, Alexandra, Per B. Brockhoff & Rune H. B. Christensen. 2017. lmerTest package: Tests in linear mixed effects models. Journal of Statistical Software 82(13). 1–26. https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v082.i13.Search in Google Scholar
Labov, William. 1963. The social motivation of a sound change. Word 19. 273–309. https://doi.org/10.1080/00437956.1963.11659799.Search in Google Scholar
Maekawa, Kikuo. 2004. Design, compilation, and some preliminary analyses of the Corpus of Spontaneous Japanese. In Kikuo Maekawa & Kiyoko Yoneyama (eds.), Spontaneous speech: Data and analysis, 87–108. Tokyo: The National Institute of Japanese Language.Search in Google Scholar
Maekawa, Kikuo, Makoto Yamazaki, Toshinobu Ogiso, Takehiko Maruyama, Hideki Ogura, Wakako Kashino, Hanae Koiso, Masaya Yamaguchi, Makiro Tanaka & Yasuharu Den. 2014. Balanced corpus of contemporary written Japanese. Language Resources and Evaluation 48(2). 345–371. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10579-013-9261-0.Search in Google Scholar
Matsumura, Akira. 1951. Edogo Tokyōgo no kenkyū [Studies of the languages of Edo and Tokyo], Revised ed. Tokyo: Tokyodo.Search in Google Scholar
Morimoto, Toshiyuki. 2013. “Kare wa kanojo ga suki da” no aimaisei: Aimaisei o motarasu bunpōronteki oyobi imironteki soin ni tsuite [Ambiguity in “Kare wa kanojo ga sukida”: On the grammatical and semantic actors]. Aichi shukutoku daigaku ronshuu: Kooryuu bunka gakubu hen 3. 59–68.Search in Google Scholar
Nakamura, Michio. 1957. Gendaigo no keikō [Tendencies in the contemporary language]. Tokyo: Hobunkan.Search in Google Scholar
Nambu, Satoshi, Hyun Kyung Hwang, David Y. Oshima & Masashi Nomura. 2018. The nominative/accusative alternation in Japanese and information structure. Journal of East Asian Linguistics 27(2). 141–171. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10831-018-9169-1.Search in Google Scholar
National Institute for Japanese Language and Linguistics. 2016. NINJAL parsed corpus of modern Japanese (Version August 26, 2021). National Institute for Japanese Language and Linguistics. http://npcmj.ninjal.ac.jp/ (accessed 26 August 2021).Search in Google Scholar
National Institute for Japanese Language and Linguistics. 2020. Corpus of historical Japanese (Version 2020.3). https://ccd.ninjal.ac.jp/chj/ (accessed 3 July 2020).Search in Google Scholar
Oe, Saburo. 1973. Ganbō no tai no mae de no o to ga no kōtai [The o/ga-alternation before desiderative tai in Modern Japanese]. Bungaku kenkyu 70. 1–11.Search in Google Scholar
Oshima, David Y. 2014. On the morphological status of -te, -ta, and related forms in Japanese: Evidence from accent placement. Journal of East Asian Linguistics 23(3). 233–265. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10831-014-9120-z.Search in Google Scholar
Oshima, David Y. 2021. When (not) to use the Japanese particle wa: Groundhood, contrastive topics, and grammatical functions. Language 96(4). e320–e340. https://doi.org/10.1353/lan.2021.0073.Search in Google Scholar
R Core Team. 2021. R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing. http://www.R-project.org (accessed 18 February 2022).Search in Google Scholar
Shibuya, Katsumi. 1993. Nihongo kanō hyōgen no shosō to hatten [Aspects and developments of potential expressions in Japanese]. Bulletin of Department of Literature 33(1). i–262.Search in Google Scholar
Shinya, Eiko. 1995. “Kanojo o sukida” to iu iikata: Kanjō keiyōshi no tadōsei ni tsuite [The “Kanojo o sukida” construction: On the transitivity of emotional adjectives]. Obirin ronshuu 22. 93–103.Search in Google Scholar
Sugai, Kazumi & Atsushi Naruse. 2006. Kibō hyōgen ni okeru taishō NP no kaku hyōji ni kansuru oboegaki [Remarks on the case marking of the objective NP in Japanese desiderative expressions]. Hyogo University of Teacher Education Journal 29. 49–57.Search in Google Scholar
Sugamoto, Nobuko. 1982. Transitivity and objecthood in Japanese. In Paul J. Hopper & Sandra A. Thompson (eds.), Syntax and semantics: Studies in Transitivity, vol. 15, 423–447. New York: Academic Press.10.1163/9789004368903_022Search in Google Scholar
Tamura, Suzuko. 1969. Nihongo tadōshi no kibōkei/kanōkei to joshi [The potential/desiderative forms of Japanese transitive predicates and particles. Waseda daigaku gogaku kyōiku kenkyūjo kiyō 8. 16–33.Search in Google Scholar
Tenny, Carol. 2006. Evidentiality, experiencers, and the syntax of sentience in Japanese. Journal of East Asian Linguistics 15. 245–288. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10831-006-0002-x.Search in Google Scholar
Yamada, Masahiro. 2018. Kakujoshi ‘ga’ no yōhō kakudai no yōsō: O kaku meishi (taishō) ni kasetsu suru yōhō o chūshin toshite [Aspects of expansion of usage of case particle ‘ga’: Its use for o-marked nouns (objects)]. Kokugo to Kokubungaku 95(1). 51–62.Search in Google Scholar
Yamazaki, Megumi. 2000. “… o suki da / kirai da” to iu hyōgen ni tsuite. [On the expression “… o suki da / kirai da”]. Nihon to Chūgoku kotoba no kakehashi: Saji Keizo kyōju koki kinen ronbunshū [The bridge between the Japanese and Chinese languages: A Festschrift celebrating the 70th birthday of professor Keizo Saji], 197–205. Tokyo: Kurosio Publishers.Search in Google Scholar
Yanagida, Seiji. 2016. Nihongo no rekishi 6: Shukaku joshi “ga” no sennenki [The history of the Japanese language 6: The millennium of the nominative particle “ga”]. Tokyo: Musashino Shoin.Search in Google Scholar
Yuzawa, Kokichiro. 1936. Tokugawa jidai gengo no kenkyū: Kamigata hen [A study of the Japanese language in the Tokugawa era: The Kyoto-Osaka area]. Tokyo: Toko Shoin.Search in Google Scholar
© 2022 Walter de Gruyter GmbH, Berlin/Boston