Abstract
It has been reported that prosody contributes to the identification of utterances which lack lexico-syntactic indicators of interrogativity but do have characteristic prosodic correlates (e.g. Vion and Colas 2006. Pitch cues for the recognition of yes-no questions in French. Journal of Psycholinguistics Research 35. 427–445). In Persian wh-in-situ questions, the interrogativity device (the wh-phrase) does not move to the sentence-initial position, and the pre-wh part is characterized by specific prosodic correlates (Shiamizadeh et al. 2016. Do Persian native speakers prosodically mark wh-in-situ questions? Manuscript submitted for publication). The current experiment investigates the role of prosody in the perception of Persian wh-in-situ questions as opposed to declaratives. To this end, an experiment was designed in which Persian native speakers were asked to choose the correct sentence type after hearing only the pre-wh part of a sentence. We hypothesized that prosody guides perception of wh-in-situ questions independent of wh-phrase type. The results of the experiment corroborate our hypothesis. The outcome is discussed in terms of Ohala´s frequency code, and Bolinger´s claim about the universal dichotomous association between relaxation and declarativity on the one hand and tension and interrogativity on the other hand.
References
Abedi, F., A. Moinzadeh & Z. Gharaei. 2012. WH-movement in English and Persian within the framework of government and binding theory. International Journal of Linguistics 4. 419–432.10.5296/ijl.v4i3.2325Search in Google Scholar
Adli, A. 2007. Constraint cumulativity and gradience: Wh-scrambling in Persian. Lingua 120. 2256–2294.10.1016/j.lingua.2010.01.003Search in Google Scholar
Baltazani, M. 2007. Intonation of polar questions and the location of nuclear stress in Greek. In Gussenhoven C. & T. Riad (eds.), Tones and tunes: Experimental studies in word and sentence prosody, 387–405. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.10.1515/9783110207576.2.387Search in Google Scholar
Baltazani, M., E. Kainada, A. Lengeris & K. Nicolaidis. 2015. The prenuclear field matters: Questions and statements in standard modern Greek. In The Scottish consortium for ICPhS 2015 (ed.), Proceedings of the 18th International Congress of Phonetic Sciences, 10–14. Lund: Lund University.Search in Google Scholar
Beach, C. M., W. F. Katz & A. Skowronski. 1996. Children‘s processing of prosodic cues for phrasal interpretation. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 99(2). 1148–1160.10.1121/1.414599Search in Google Scholar
Boersma, P. & D. Weenink. 2014. Praat: Doing phonetics by computer (Version 6.0.04) [Computer program]. http://www.praat.org/.Search in Google Scholar
Bolinger, D. W. 1989. Intonation and its uses. Palo Alto: Stanford University Press.10.1515/9781503623125Search in Google Scholar
Bolinger, D. 1981. The place of intonation in a discourse model. In M. C. Loulth & M. Montgomery (eds.), Studies in discourse analysis, 146-157. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.Search in Google Scholar
Carlson, K., C. Clifton & L. Frazier. 2001. Prosodic boundaries in adjunct attachment. Journal of Memory and Language 45. 58–81.10.1006/jmla.2000.2762Search in Google Scholar
Carnie, A. 2007. Syntax: A generative introduction, 2nd edn. Oxford: Blackwell.Search in Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. 1977. On wh-movement. In P. W. Culicover, T. Wasow & A. Akmajian (eds.), Formal syntax, 71–132. New York: Academic Press.Search in Google Scholar
D’Imperio, M. 2000. The role of perception in defining tonal targets and their alignment. Doctoral dissertation, Ohio State University https://etd.ohiolink.edu/!etd.send_file?accession=osu1243021045&disposition=inline.Search in Google Scholar
De Moraes, J. A. 1998. Intonation in Brasilian Portuguese. In D. Hirst & A. Di Cristo (eds.), Intonation systems: A survey of twenty languages, 179–194. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Search in Google Scholar
Di Cristo, A. & D. J. Hirst. 1993. Prosodic regularities in the surface structure of French questions. In D. House & P. Touati (eds.), Proceedings of the European Speech Communication Association Workshop on Prosody, 268–271. Lund: Lund University.Search in Google Scholar
Face, T. L. 2004. The intonation of absolute interrogatives in Castilian Spanish. Southwest Journal of Linguistics 23(2). 65–79.Search in Google Scholar
Face, T. L. 2005. F0 peak height and the perception of sentence type in Castilian Spanish. Revista International de Linguistica Iberoamericana 2. 49–65.Search in Google Scholar
Gorjian, B., M. Naqhizadeh & P. Shahramiri. 2012. Making interrogative sentences in English and Persian language: A contrastive analysis approach. Journal of Comparative Linguistics and Literature 2. 120–124.Search in Google Scholar
Grosjean, F. 1983. How long is the sentence? Prediction and prosody in the on-line processing of language. Linguistics 21. 501–529.10.1515/ling.1983.21.3.501Search in Google Scholar
Grosjean, F. 1996. Using prosody to predict the end of sentences in English and French: Normal and brain damaged subjects. Language and Cognitive Processes 11. 1–2.10.1080/016909696387231Search in Google Scholar
Haan, J. 2003. Speaking of questions: An exploration of Dutch question intonation. Doctoral dissertation, Leiden University. www.lotpublications.nl/Documents/52_fulltext.pdf.Search in Google Scholar
IBM SPSS. 2012. IBM SPSS. IBM Software Business Analytics.Search in Google Scholar
Kahnemuyipour, A. 2009. The syntax of sentential stress. Oxford: Oxford University Press.10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199219230.001.0001Search in Google Scholar
Karimi, S. 2005. A minimalist approach to scrambling: Evidence from Persian. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.10.1515/9783110199796Search in Google Scholar
Karimi, S. & A. Taleghani. 2007. Wh-movement, interpretation, and optionality in Persian. In S. Karimi, V. Samiian & W. K. Wilkins (eds.), Phrasal and clausal architecture: Syntactic derivation and interpretation, 167–187. Amsterdam: Benjamins.10.1075/la.101.09karSearch in Google Scholar
Liberman, M. & J. Pierrehumbert. 1984. Intonational in-variance under changes in pitch range and length. In M. Aronoff & R. T. Oehrle (eds.), Language sound structure, 157–233. Cambridge: MIT Press.Search in Google Scholar
Lotfi, A. R. 2003. Persian Wh-riddles. In C. Boeckx & K.K. Grohmann (eds.), Multiple wh-Fronting, 161–186. Amsterdam: Benjamins.10.1075/la.64.11lotSearch in Google Scholar
Megerdoomian, K. & S. Ganjavi. 2000. Against optional wh-Movement. In V. Samiian (eds.), Proceedings of the Western Conference on Linguistics: WECOL, 358–370. Frenso: California University.Search in Google Scholar
Mirsaeedi, A. 2006. Wh-movement in Persian language IsfahanUniversity of Isfahan MA thesis.Search in Google Scholar
Ohala, J. 1984. An ethological perspective on common cross-language utilization of F0 of voice. Phonetica 41. 1–16.10.1159/000261706Search in Google Scholar
Petrone, C. 2008. Le rôle de la variabilité phonétique dans la représentation des contours intonatifs et de leur sens. Doctoral dissertation, Université de Provence https://tipa.revues.org/410.10.4000/tipa.410Search in Google Scholar
Petrone, C. & M. D’Imperio. 2008. edited by P. A. Basbosa, S. Madureira & C. Reis Proceedings of the Fourth Conference on Speech Prosody, São PauloCapes Tonal structure and constituency in Neapolitan Italian: Evidence for the accentual phrase in statements and questions. 301-304.Search in Google Scholar
Petrone, C. & M. D’Imperio. 2011. From tones to tunes: Effects of the f0 prenuclear region in the perception of Neapolitan statements and questions. In S. Frota, G. Elordieta & P. Prieto (eds.), Prosodic categories: Production, perception and comprehension, 207–230. Dordrecht: Springer.10.1007/978-94-007-0137-3_9Search in Google Scholar
Petrone, C. & O. Niebuhr. 2014. On the intonation of German intonation questions: The role of the prenuclear region. Language and Speech 57(1). 108–146.10.1177/0023830913495651Search in Google Scholar
Psychology Software Tools. 2012. E-Prime. Pittsburg, PA.Search in Google Scholar
Rialland, A. (2004). Paper presented at the International Conference on Tone and Intonation, Greece, Santorini A typology of question prosody in African languages.Search in Google Scholar
Rietveld, A.C.M. & C. Gussenhoven. 1987. Perceived speech rate and intonation. Journal of Phonetics 15. 273–285.10.1016/S0095-4470(19)30571-6Search in Google Scholar
Rietveld, T. & P. Vermillion. 2003. Cues for perceived pitch register. Phonetica 60. 261–272.10.1159/000076376Search in Google Scholar
Sensui, H. 1995. Percepción de la entonación interrogativa del español: Un estudio experimental. Sophia Lingüística 38. 1–23.Search in Google Scholar
Shiamizadeh, Z., J. Caspers & N. O. Schiller. 2016. Do Persian native speakers prosodically mark wh-in-situ questions?. Manuscript submitted for publication.Search in Google Scholar
Snedeker, J. & J. Trueswell. 2003. Using prosody to avoid ambiguity: Effects of speaker awareness and referential context. Journal of Memory and Language 48(1). 103–130.10.1016/S0749-596X(02)00519-3Search in Google Scholar
Stanislaw, H. & N. Todorov. 1999. Calculation of signal detection theory measures. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments and Computers 31. 137–149.10.3758/BF03207704Search in Google Scholar
Toosarvandani, M. 2008. Wh-movement and syntax of sluicing. Journal of Linguistics 44. 677–722.10.1017/S0022226708005367Search in Google Scholar
Truckenbrodt, H., F. Sandalo & B. Abaurre. 2009. Elements of Brazilian Portuguese intonation. Journal of Portuguese Linguistics 8(1). 75–114.10.5334/jpl.122Search in Google Scholar
Van Heuven, V. & J. Haan. 2000. Phonetic correlates of statement versus questions intonation in Dutch. In A. Botinis (eds.), Intonation, analysis, modeling and technology, 119–143. Dordrecht: Kluwer.10.1007/978-94-011-4317-2_6Search in Google Scholar
Van Heuven, V. & E. Van Zanten. 2005. Speech rate as a secondary prosodic characteristic of polarity questions in three languages. Speech Communication 47. 87–99.10.1016/j.specom.2005.05.010Search in Google Scholar
Vion, M. & A. Colas. 2006. Pitch cues for the recognition of yes-no questions in French. Journal of Psycholinguistics Research 35. 427–445.10.1007/s10936-006-9023-xSearch in Google Scholar
Appendix
Structure and word constituents of the sentences from which the stimuli of this experiment is extracted. Only the pre-wh part (subject and the adverb) comprise the stimuli of this experiment.
Subject | Adverb | 3rd constituent of the sentence | Verb | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Wh-phrase | DWCa | |||
1) mohæmædʔæmin | 1) pæriruz | 1) tʃi (what) | Inanimate DOb | tæʔmir kærdæn |
(Mohamadamin) | (two days ago) | 1) kæfʃ (shoe) | (repair) | |
2) kif (bag) | ||||
2) mohæmædʔæli | 2) ki (who) | Animate DO | dæʔvæt kærdæn | |
(Mohamadali) | 1) jɑs (Yas) | (invite) | ||
2) nɑz (Naz) | ||||
3) kodʒɑ (where) | Adjuct of Placec | bɑzi kærdæn (play) | ||
1) dʒængæl (jungle) | ||||
2) kutʃe (street) | ||||
4) kej (when) | Adjunct of Time | ʃenɑ kærdæn (swim) | ||
1) zohr (noon) | ||||
2) ʔæsr (afternoon) | ||||
5) tʃetori (how) | Adjunct of Manner | sohbæt kærdæn (talk) | ||
1) bɑ ʔænduh (sadly) | ||||
2) bɑ deqæt (carefully) |
aDWC refers (declarative wh-phrase counterpart) to the categories which replace the wh-phrase in declaratives.
bDO refers to direct object. The object marker “ra” occurs after direct object in declaratives and wh-in-situ questions.
cThe preposition “tu” which means “at” precedes the adjunct of place in declaratives but not in wh-in-situ questions.
Descriptive statistics of the prosodic correlates of the stimuli across sentence types.
Variable | M SD (Decl) | M SD (Wh-q) | N |
---|---|---|---|
F0 onset (ERB) | 5.082 | 5.928 | 40 |
1.303 | 0.774 | ||
Subject Pitch Excursion (ERB) | 1.025 | 1.740 | 40 |
0.420 | 0.354 | ||
Adverb Pitch Excursion (ERB) | 0.622 | 1.430 | 40 |
0.109 | 0.222 | ||
Slope of regression line of the pre-wh part (ERB/sec) | 0.143 | 0.954 | 40 |
0.102 | 0.211 | ||
F0 mean of the pre-wh part (ERB) | 4.903 | 5.666 | 40 |
1.120 | 0.951 | ||
Duration of the pre-wh part (in sec) | 1.360 | 1.043 | 40 |
0.072 | 0.069 |
© 2017 Walter de Gruyter GmbH, Berlin/Boston