Skip to content
Licensed Unlicensed Requires Authentication Published by De Gruyter Mouton August 10, 2018

Loci and locality of informational effects on phonetic implementation

Robert Daland EMAIL logo and Kie Zuraw
From the journal Linguistics Vanguard

Abstract

Recent evidence suggests that the phonetic realization of linguistic units is sensitive to informational context. For example, the duration of a word is shorter when it is probable given the following word. Word-specific phonetic variation is unexpected according to modular/feedforward models. We consider various challenges to identifying the loci of informational effects on phonetic implementation – do they arise in production, perception, memory, or some combination? Section 2 addresses a theoretical issue: what are the right measure(s) of predictability/informativity? An urgent direction for future work is to understand what kinds of context matter and why. Section 3 reviews second-mention reduction and other non-local discourse effects, which strongly suggest a production locus (rather than arising in speech perception or memory). Important future directions include modeling discourse/topic in corpus studies, and experimentally assessing the role of nonlocal context in perception and memory. Section 4 addresses the role of computational modeling. We call for integrated, implemented end-to-end models which include speech perception, lexical representation, and speech production components.

Acknowledgement

We wish to acknowledge Shigeto Kawahara and an anonymous reviewer for their thoughtful critique of a previous version of this manuscript.

References

Altmann, Eduardo G., Janet B. Pierrehumbert & Adilson E. Motter. 2009. Beyond word frequency: Bursts, lulls, and scaling in the temporal distributions of words. PLoS ONE 4(11). e7678. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0007678.10.1371/journal.pone.0007678Search in Google Scholar

Anderson, Anne H. & Barbara Howarth. 2002. Referential form and word duration in video-mediated and face-to-face dialogues. In Johan Bos, Mary Ellen Foster & Colin Matheson (Eds.), Proceedings of the Sixth Workshop on the Semantics and Pragmatics of Dialogue (EDILOG 2002), Edinburgh, UK, 4–6 September 2002, pp. 13–20. Edinburgh: Cognitive Science Centre, University of Edinburgh.Search in Google Scholar

Baker, Rachel E. & Ann R. Bradlow. 2009. Variability in word duration as a function of probability, speech style, and prosody. Language and Speech 52(4). 391–413.10.1177/0023830909336575Search in Google Scholar

Bard, Ellen G., Anne H. Anderson, Catherine Sotillo, Matthew Aylett, Gwyneth Doherty-Sneddon & Alison Newlands. 2000. Controlling the intelligibility of referring expressions in dialogue. Journal of Memory and Language 42. 1–22.10.1006/jmla.1999.2667Search in Google Scholar

Bard, Ellen G., A. Lowe & Gerry T.M. Altmann. 1989. The effects of repetition on words in recorded dictations. In Jean-Pierre Tubach & Joseph Mariani (Eds.), Proceedings of EUROSPEECH-1989 First European Conference on Speech Communication and Technology (EUROSPEECH ’89), Paris, France, September 27–29, 1989 (pp 573–576). Paris: European Speech Communication Association.10.21437/Eurospeech.1989-301Search in Google Scholar

Bishop, Jason. 2012. Information structural expectations in the perception of prosodic prominence. In G. Elordieta & P. Prieto (Eds.) Prosody and Meaning (Interface Explorations). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.10.1515/9783110261790.239Search in Google Scholar

Bell, Alan, Jason M. Brenier, Michelle Gregory, Cynthia Girand & Dan Jurafsky. 2009. Predictability effects on durations of content and function words in conversational English. Journal of Memory and Language 60(1). 92–111. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2008.06.003.10.1016/j.jml.2008.06.003Search in Google Scholar

Blei, David M., Andrew Y. Ng & Michael I. Jordan. 2003. Latent Dirichlet allocation. Journal of Machine Learning Research 3. 993–1022.Search in Google Scholar

Braun, Bettina, Greg Kochanski, Esther Grabe & Burton S. Rosner. 2006. Evidence for attractors in English intonation. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 119(6). 4006–4015. http://dx.doi.org/10.1121/1.2195267.10.1121/1.2195267Search in Google Scholar

Bybee, Joan. 2001. Phonology and language use. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.10.1017/CBO9780511612886Search in Google Scholar

Carbary, Kathleen, Meredith Brown, Christine Gunlogson, Joyce M. McDonough, Aleksandra Fazlipour & Michael K. Tanenhaus. 2015. Anticipatory deaccenting in language comprehension. Language, Cognition, and Neuroscience 30(1–2). 197–211. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/23273798.2014.885534.10.1080/23273798.2014.885534Search in Google Scholar

Church, Kenneth. 2000. Empirical estimates of adaptation: The chance of two Noriega’s is closer to p/2 than p2. COLING, pp. 173–179.10.3115/990820.990847Search in Google Scholar

Cohen Priva, Uriel. 2008. Using information content to predict phone deletion. In Proceedings of the 27th West Coast Conference on Formal Linguistics, edited by Natasha Abner & Jason Bishop, 90–98. Somerville, MA, USA: Cascadilla Proceedings Project.Search in Google Scholar

Cohen Priva, Uriel. 2012. Sign and signal: Deriving linguistic generalizations from information utility. Unpublished dissertation, Stanford University.Search in Google Scholar

Cohen Priva, Uriel. 2017. Informativity and the actuation of lenition. Language 93(3). 569–597.10.1353/lan.2017.0037Search in Google Scholar

Daland, Robert. 2013. Variation in child-directed speech: A case study of manner class frequencies. Journal of Child Language 40(5). 1091–1122.10.1017/S0305000912000372Search in Google Scholar

Daland, Robert, Mira Oh & Syejeong Kim. 2015. When in doubt, read the instructions: Orthographic effects in loanword adaptation. Lingua 159. 70–92. doi: 10.1016/j.lingua.2015.03.002.10.1016/j.lingua.2015.03.002Search in Google Scholar

Dupoux, Emmanuel, Kazuhiko Kakehi, Yuki Hirose, Christophe Pallier & Jacques Mehler. 1999. Epenthetic vowels in Japanese: A perceptual illusion? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 25(6). 1568–1578.10.1037/0096-1523.25.6.1568Search in Google Scholar

Ernestus, Mirjam, Haarald R. Baayen & Robert Schreuder. 2002. The recognition of reduced word forms. Brain and Language 81(1–3). 162–173. doi: 10.1006/brln.2001.2514.10.1006/brln.2001.2514Search in Google Scholar

Fink, Angela & Matthew Goldrick. 2015. The influence of word retrieval and planning on phonetic variation: Implications for exemplar models. Linguistics Vanguard 1. 215–225. doi: 10.1515/lingvan-2015-1003.10.1515/lingvan-2015-1003Search in Google Scholar

Flege, James E. 2007. Language contact in bilingualism: Phonetic system interactions. In Jennifer Cole & Jose Hualde (Eds.), Laboratory Phonology 9, 353–380. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Search in Google Scholar

Fosler-Lussier, Eric & Nelson Morgan. 1999. Effects of speaking rate and word predictability on conversational pronunciations. Speech Communication 29. 137–158.10.1016/S0167-6393(99)00035-7Search in Google Scholar

Fowler, Carol A. 1988. Differential shortening of repeated context words produced in various communicative contexts. Language and Speech 31. 307–319.10.1177/002383098803100401Search in Google Scholar

Fowler, Carol A., Elena T. Levy & Julie M. Brown. 1997. Reductions of spoken words in certain discourse contexts. Journal of Memory and Language 37. 24–40.10.1006/jmla.1996.2504Search in Google Scholar

German, James. 2009. Prosodic strategies for negotiating reference in discourse. Doctoral thesis, Department of Linguistics, Northwestern University.Search in Google Scholar

Hale, John. 2003. The information conveyed by words in sentences. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 32(2). 101–123. doi: 10.1023/A:1022492123056.10.1023/A:1022492123056Search in Google Scholar

Hall, Kathleen, Elizabeth Hume, T. Florian Jaeger & Andrew Wedel. Under review. The message shapes phonology.Search in Google Scholar

Kello, Christopher T. & David C. Plaut. 2004. A neural network model of the articulatory-acoustic forward mapping trained on recordings of articulatory parameters. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 116. 2354–2364.10.1121/1.1715112Search in Google Scholar

Kirchner, Robert, Roger K. Moore & Tsung-Ying Chen. 2010. Computing phonological generalization over real speech exemplars. Journal of Phonetics 38(4). 540–547. doi: 10.1016/j.wocn.2010.07.005.10.1016/j.wocn.2010.07.005Search in Google Scholar

Lam, Tuan Q. & Duane G. Watson. 2015. Repetition reduction: Lexical repetition in the absence of referent repetition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 40(3). 829–843. doi: 10.1037/a0035780.10.1037/a0035780Search in Google Scholar

Levelt, William J. M., Ardi Roelofs, & Antje S. Meyer. 1999. A theory of lexical access in speech production. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 22. 1–75.10.3115/992628.992631Search in Google Scholar

Levy, Roger, Klinton Bicknell, Tim Slattery & Keith Rayner. 2009. Eye movement evidence that readers maintain and act on uncertainty about past linguistic input. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 106(50). 21086–21090. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0907664106.10.1073/pnas.0907664106Search in Google Scholar

Luce, R. Duncan 1959. Individual choice behavior: A theoretical analysis. New York: Wiley.Search in Google Scholar

McClelland, James L. & Jeffrey L. Elman. 1986. The TRACE model of speech perception. Cognitive Psychology 18. 1–86.10.1016/0010-0285(86)90015-0Search in Google Scholar

Munson, Benjamin. 2001. Phonological pattern frequency and speech production in children and adults. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 44. 778–792.10.1044/1092-4388(2001/061)Search in Google Scholar

Niyogi, Partha. 2006. The computational nature of language learning and evolution. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.10.7551/mitpress/2024.001.0001Search in Google Scholar

Piantadosi, Steven T., Harry Tily & Edward Gibson. 2011. Word lengths are optimized for efficient communication. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 108(9). 3526–3529. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1012551108.10.1073/pnas.1012551108Search in Google Scholar

Pierrehumbert, Janet B. 2001. Exemplar dynamics: Word frequency, lenition, and contrast. In Joan Bybee & Paul Hopper (Eds.) Frequency effects and the emergence of lexical structure, 137–157. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.10.1075/tsl.45.08pieSearch in Google Scholar

Pierrehumbert, Janet B. 2002. Word-specific phonetics. Laboratory Phonology VII, 101–139. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.10.1515/9783110197105.1.101Search in Google Scholar

Pierrehumbert, Janet B. 2006. The next toolkit. Journal of Phonetics 34(4). 516–530. doi: 10.1016/j.wocn.2006.06.003.10.1016/j.wocn.2006.06.003Search in Google Scholar

Plaut, David C. & Christopher T. Kello. 1999. The emergence of phonology from the interplay of speech comprehension and production: A distributed connectionist approach. In Brian MacWhinney (Ed.), The emergence of language, 381–415. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Search in Google Scholar

Roelofs, Ardi. 2005. From Popper to Lakatos: A case for cumulative computational modeling. In Anne Cutler (Ed.), Twenty-first century psycholinguistics: Four cornerstones, 313–330. Hillsdale, NJ: LEA.Search in Google Scholar

Roy, Brandon C., Michael C. Frank, & Deb Roy. 2012. Relating activity contexts to early word learning in dense longitudinal data. Proceedings of the 34th Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society. Sapporo, Japan.Search in Google Scholar

Schreuder, Robert & Haarald R. Baayen. 1995. Modeling morphological processing. In Laurie B. Feldman (Ed.), Morphological aspects of language processing, 131–154. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Search in Google Scholar

Shaw, Jason & Shigeto Kawahara. 2017. Effects of surprisal and entropy on vowel duration in Japanese. Language and Speech. doi:10.1177/0023830917737331.10.1177/0023830917737331Search in Google Scholar

Seyfarth, Scott. 2014. Word informativity influences acoustic duration: Effects of contextual predictability on lexical representation. Cognition 133(1). 140–155. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.06.013.10.1016/j.cognition.2014.06.013Search in Google Scholar

Strauss, Ted. J., Harlan D. Harris & James S. Magnuson. 2007. jTRACE: A reimplementation and extension of the TRACE model of speech perception and spoken word recognition. Behavior Research Methods 39. 19–30.10.3758/BF03192840Search in Google Scholar

Tanaka, Yu. 2015. The perceptual basis of the skewed distributions of Japanese palatalized consonants. In Thuy Bui & Deniz Ozyldz (Eds.) Proceedings of the North East Linguistic Society (NELS) 45(3). 131–144.Search in Google Scholar

Tomasello, Michael & Daniel Stahl. 2004. Sampling children’s spontaneous speech: How much is enough? Journal of Child Language 31(1). 101–121.10.1017/S0305000903005944Search in Google Scholar

Vajrabhaya, Prakiwan & Vsevolod Kapatsinsky. Under review. First time’s the charm: First-mention lengthening as an automated act.Search in Google Scholar

Van Son, Rob J. J. H. & Louis C. W. Pols. 2003. How efficient is speech? Proceedings of the Institute of Phonetic Sciences (Instituut voor Fonetische Wetenschappen, Universiteit van Amsterdam) 25: 171–184.Search in Google Scholar

Zipf, George. 1935. The psychobiology of language: An introduction to dynamic philology. Cambridge, MA: M.I.T. Press.Search in Google Scholar

Received: 2017-05-05
Accepted: 2018-01-02
Published Online: 2018-08-10

©2018 Walter de Gruyter GmbH, Berlin/Boston

Downloaded on 8.12.2022 from https://www.degruyter.com/document/doi/10.1515/lingvan-2017-0045/html
Scroll Up Arrow