Abstract
Brain-friendly learning is a new catchphrase in school and university instructional practice. However, it often escapes the notice of the teachers and learners involved that neurodidactics is not simply a plausible concept – it can also be a myth if applied incorrectly. Numerous international studies show that both pre-service and in-service teachers as well as university educators endorse misconceptions on the topic of learning and the brain and orient their didactic conception on so-called neuromyths. This paper presents nine neuromyths on the topic of learning and memory. Based on a review of the current research, we discuss what determines their emergence and prevalence, to what extent neuromyths pose a problem for practice, and why and how both neurodidactics and neuromyths should be made an object of university instruction.
Zusammenfassung
Gehirngerechtes Lernen gilt als neues Schlagwort in der schulischen und hochschuldidaktischen Praxis. Was dabei häufig aus dem Blick der engagierten Lehrenden und Lernenden gerät, ist, dass Neurodidaktik nicht nur ein plausibles Konzept, sondern falsch angewendet auch ein Mythos sein kann. Zahlreiche internationale Studien zeigen, dass sowohl angehende und praktizierende Lehrkräfte als auch Hochschullehrende Fehlvorstellungen zum Thema Gehirn und Lernen Glauben schenken und ihre didaktische Konzeption an sogenannten Neuromythen ausrichten. In diesem Beitrag werden neun Neuromythen zum Thema Lernen und Gedächtnis vorgestellt. Auf Basis einer Darstellung des aktuellen Forschungsstands wird erläutert, was ihre Entstehung und Verbreitung bedingt, inwiefern Neuromythen ein Problem für die Praxis darstellen und warum bzw. wie sowohl Neurodidaktik als auch Neuromythen zum Gegenstand universitärer Lehre gemacht werden sollten.
Introduction and objectives
In recent years, insights from the field of brain research have launched a downright neuro-boom reflected not only in numerous publications but also in transfer attempts such as neuromarketing, neuroarchitecture, neuromanagement, and neurodidactics (cf. e. g., Häusel, 2008; Herreros, 2012; Herrmann, 2009; Metzger, 2018). Teachers especially show great interest in neuroscientific research findings and consider it useful to incorporate them when designing their instruction (Dekker et al., 2012). Brain-friendly learning [1] is seen as a new magic spell, not only in schools but also in university instruction (Folta-Schoofs and Ostermann, 2019). Nevertheless, (pre-service) teachers and university instructors, the alleged experts on learning, still endorse numerous neuroscientific misconceptions and partially orient their instructional practice on so-called neuromyths (e. g., Dekker et al., 2012; Gleichgerrcht et al., 2015). The term neuromyths can be traced back to the neurosurgeon Alan Crockard, who used it in the 1980s to refer to scientifically inappropriate understandings of the brain in medical culture (Howard-Jones, 2010). The Organization for Economic Co-operation and Development (OECD, 2002) defines neuromyths as “misconception[s] generated by a misunderstanding, a misreading, or a misquoting of facts scientifically established (by brain research) to make a case for use of brain research in education and other contexts” (p. 111). Neuromyths have been identified with respect to numerous topics, including indications of specific learning difficulties such as dyslexia (Macdonald et al., 2017) or the influence of nutrition (Dekker et al., 2012) and music (Düvel et al., 2017) on the brain. This paper focuses on neuromyths concerning the topic of learning and memory. We discuss what research findings on neuromyths currently exist and what problems and objects for university instruction result from them.
Neuromyths on the topic of learning and memory
Grospietsch and Mayer (2019) identified 11 neuromyths on the topic of learning and memory. The study's scientific content analysis showed that each of these misconceptions is based on a kernel of truth (= scientific term/research finding) and morphs over a chain of erroneous conclusions into a no-longer-scientifically-correct implication for teaching and learning (= neuromyth). Insights from neuroscience and cognitive psychology form the starting points for each fallacious line of argument. Table 1 compares the kernel of truth and neuromyth for the nine neuromyths addressed in this paper to provide an overview of the problem of neuroscientific research findings being inaccurately transferred to teaching and learning. Three concrete examples of the individual errors in transfer involved as well as the scientific refutation of the neuromyths learning while you sleep, logic in the left hemisphere/creativity in the right, and that we only use 10% of the brain can be found in Grospietsch and Mayer (2019).
Scientific kernels of truth and the neuromyths resulting from them.
Scientific kernel of truth | Neuromyth |
---|---|
Perception preferences
Learners exhibit preferences for receiving information in a specific mode. |
Existence of learning styles
Individuals learn better when they receive information corresponding to their learning style (e. g., auditory, visual, haptic, or intellectual). |
Crossover in neural pathways
Neural pathways link the left brain hemisphere to the right side of the body and vice versa. |
Effectiveness of Brain Gym
Coordination exercises (e. g., cross-body movements) can improve the interaction between the left and right brain hemispheres and thus learning and/or intelligence. |
Existence of cortical regions
The cerebrum is made up of different cortical regions subject to a functional division of labor. |
Specific storage locations (hard drive)
The brain works like a hard drive. Information is stored in specific locations (e. g., in the center for math). |
Hemispheric dominance
One brain hemisphere is more strongly involved in a certain cognitive process than the other. |
Differences due to hemispheric use
Each person uses their left and right brain hemispheres to different degrees, which explains differences between learners. This hemispheric dominance needs to be taken into account. |
Brain development
Neural cell connections enormously increase in the first years of life. |
Best learning before age 3
Learners are most receptive to learning processes from birth until age 3. |
Hemispheric asymmetry
Two cerebral hemispheres exist that are not completely identical, both anatomically and functionally. |
Logic on the left/creativity on the right
Creative thought processes engage the right brain hemisphere, while logical thought processes engage the left. Deliberate effort must be made to equally engage both brain hemispheres. |
Sensitive phases in child development
There are sensitive phases in childhood during which certain things can be learned more easily and in which isolation from stimuli can lead to irreversible damage (e. g., language acquisition). |
Critical time periods for learning
Children must be presented with as many good stimuli as possible during this time window that then closes irrevocably so that their learning will not be impaired throughout their life, as this cannot be corrected through education. |
Brain activity
Imaging techniques make it possible to measure which brain regions are involved in a mental or physical activity. |
Only use 10% of the brain
We only use the 10% of our brain regions highlighted in images (e. g., fMRI) and thus only a fraction of our mental capacity. |
Consolidation
Nighttime restructuring processes can lead to the gaining of new insights during sleep. |
Learning while you sleep
Completely new content can be learned during sleep via the acoustic channel (e. g., audio recordings of vocabulary lists). |
-
Note: This table was created based on a summary of the current state of theory on neuromyths as well as supplementary literature research: Bear et al. (2018), Biswal et al. (2010), Carter (2014), Dekker et al. (2012), de Lussanet and Osse (2012), Gais and Born (2004), Grospietsch and Mayer (2019); Höffler et al. (2017), Jäncke (2013), OECD (2002).
Numerous studies (cf. Figure 1) show that pre-service and in-service teachers as well as university educators exhibit great interest in neuroscience but are simultaneously unable to differentiate between neuromyths and neurofacts. There is a general tendency to endorse neuroscientific statements about the topic of learning and memory – regardless of whether or not they are neuromyths (Grospietsch and Mayer, 2019). University professors and instructors who train future teachers endorse neuromyths at slightly lower rates than (pre-service) teachers (Gleichgerrcht et al., 2015; van Dijk and Lane, 2018). In-service teachers, in turn, endorse neuromyths a little less frequently than pre-service teachers (Canbulat and Kiriktas, 2017). However, Zhang et al. (2019) and Horvath et al. (2018) demonstrate that even headmasters and award-winning teachers endorse neuromyths at high levels.

Overview of existing studies on neuromyths among pre-service teachers, in-service teachers, and university educators.
Research on the prevalence of neuromyths can be summarized as mostly consistent, with the exception of a few cultural differences between countries. Many neuromyths on learning and memory are endorsed to a high degree. Myths concerning the effectiveness of Brain Gym and existence of learning styles are particularly widespread and have found their way into learning guides and educational programs (Grospietsch and Mayer, 2019; Pasquinelli, 2012). Research findings on the factors determining the endorsement of neuromyths are more diverse. Ferrero et al. (2016) conclude that reading educational magazines increases beliefs in neuromyths. Conversely, Düvel et al. (2017) show that reading a large number of educational books, magazines, and websites reduces endorsement of neuromyths. Research by Macdonald et al. (2017) and Ferrero et al. (2016) indicates that reading scientific journals reduces beliefs in neuromyths. In contrast, Gleichgerrcht et al. (2015) determine that neither neuroscientific nor popular science articles sufficiently reduce endorsement of neuromyths. Macdonald et al. (2017) could show that people with high levels of neuroscientific knowledge endorse neuromyths to a lesser degree than teachers and the general public. Papadatou-Pastou et al. (2017) emphasize that general knowledge about the brain is the best “safeguard against believing in neuromyths” (p. 1). This result is corroborated by van Dijk and Lane (2018). However, in numerous studies, teachers with high levels of scientifically appropriate conceptions of the brain prove to be more susceptible to neuromyths (e. g., Dekker et al., 2012; Ferrero et al., 2016; Papadatou-Pastou et al., 2017). Research findings concerning personal characteristics are also inconsistent. The majority of studies show that age, gender, professional experience, teaching subject, school type, school location (urban/rural) and participation in professional development courses are not correlated with the endorsement of either neuromyths or scientifically appropriate conceptions about the brain (e. g., Dekker et al., 2012; Karakus et al., 2015; Papadatou-Pastou et al., 2017; Rato et al., 2013). Macdonald et al. (2017) conclude that being younger, having a university degree, and attending neuroscience courses reduce but do not eliminate endorsement of neuromyths. The latter result is corroborated by Canbulat and Kiriktas (2017) as well as Ruhaak and Cook (2018). Four studies have found an association between endorsement of neuromyths and gender. In two studies, female teachers are more likely to endorse neuromyths (Dündar and Gündüz, 2016; Ferrero et al., 2016), but they outperform male subjects in two other studies (Canbulat and Kiriktas, 2017; Macdonald et al., 2017). In sum, we primarily know one thing: pre-service and in-service teachers as well as university educators endorse numerous neuromyths on the topic of learning and memory. The leading determinants of these beliefs and how they can be effectively reduced still remain open questions.
Neuromyths' resistance as a problem of university instruction
Although the current research literature on neuromyths calls for integrating more neuroscience into teacher training (e. g., Howard-Jones, 2014), this alone does not seem to be sufficient to professionalize pre-service teachers' misconceptions on the topic of learning and memory. According to Dündar and Gündüz (2016), pre-service science teachers significantly outperform pre-service teachers of other subjects, whereas studies by Macdonald et al. (2017) and Im et al. (2018) indicate that mere enrollment in neuroscience or psychology courses at university does not sufficiently reduce endorsement of neuromyths. A study by Grospietsch and Mayer (2019) showed that even pre-service biology teachers, who receive instruction in neuroscientific content during their studies (e. g., courses in human biology and animal physiology), endorse neuromyths to a great extent. As shown in Figure 2, all nine misconceptions on the topic of learning and memory were endorsed by more than half of pre-service biology teachers. Participants at different stages of their training (first-year students, more advanced students, and graduates enrolled in practical teacher preparation) differed only with respect to their endorsement of scientifically appropriate conceptions, but not in their endorsement of neuromyths (Grospietsch and Mayer, 2019).

Pre-service biology teachers' (N = 550) endorsement of nine neuromyths on the topic of learning and memory (Grospietsch and Mayer, 2019).
Given that biology teachers need to not only address the topic of learning and the brain as instructional content but also use it to guide their students' learning processes, the conceptions of pre-service biology teachers – up until the end of their practical training phase – must be described as deficient. The results of another study (Grospietsch and Mayer, 2018) show that even a university course conveying and closely interlinking professional knowledge from the fields of cognitive psychology, neuroscience, and biology didactics on the topic of learning and the brain is insufficient for students to critically engage with neuromyths. The results of Grospietsch and Mayer's (2019) study indicate that neuromyths exist in parallel to accurate professional knowledge and beliefs about neuroscience and learning and can prove to be resistant to conventional teacher education. This means that even after acquiring professional knowledge, university students are released into practice with misconceptions. According to Horvath et al. (2018), there is still a lack of studies proving that endorsement of neuromyths negatively affects teachers' effectiveness, students' learning performance or their perceived self-efficacy. However, Lethaby and Harries (2016) and Blanchette Sarrasin et al. (2019) highlight that many teachers who endorse neuromyths employ practices linked to these misconceptions in their instruction (with pre-school teachers doing so most frequently, followed by primary school teachers and then secondary school teachers). This is problematic on the one hand because it could lead teachers to pass on incorrect cognitive psychology/neuroscience content and/or ineffective learning strategies to their students. On the other hand, the education system's “money, time and effort” (Dekker et al., 2012, p. 1) could be wasted and both teachers and learners are deprived of the opportunity to expend these resources on more effective theories and methods (e. g., teaching learning strategies or cognitive activation) (Grospietsch and Mayer, 2019). Ruhaak and Cook (2018) show that accurate beliefs about neuromyths are associated with a higher probability of employing effective, rather than ineffective, neuromyth-based instructional practices. Hence, developing university instruction programs for pre-service teachers as well as professional development opportunities for in-service teachers that clarify neuromyths scientifically and professionalize them sustainably is of great relevance.
Brain-friendly learning and neuromyths as an object of university instruction
Related to the goal of lifelong learning in a rapidly developing society, a professional understanding of learning is important for both teachers and learners. University education should be capable of providing students with an appropriate conception of learning. Langfeldt and Nieder (2004) summarize, with respect to teacher education, that around one-third of students' learning concepts prove to be resistant to change over the course of their studies and cannot be sufficiently professionalized into more pedagogically-desirable concepts. Studies on neuromyths show that (pre-service) teachers encounter neuromyths and related practices in their academic and practical training and professional development (Blanchette Sarrasin et al., 2019; Howard-Jones et al., 2009; Lethaby and Harries, 2016; Ruhaak and Cook, 2018; Tardif et al., 2015). Although (pre-service) teachers primarily refer to TV, the internet and popular science magazines in their research (Ferrero et al., 2016; Rato et al., 2013), university instructors should view their teaching as a significant opportunity to build up accurate neuroscientific knowledge, a well-founded conception of neurodidactic, and an evidence-based understanding of learning (Grospietsch and Mayer, 2019). Thus, in light of the reported research findings, it seems necessary for the disciplines involved (neurobiology, cognitive psychology, education science, and subject and university didactics) not to leave the field to pop science but rather to actively counter the misunderstanding, misreading, or misquoting of facts scientifically established by brain research to make a case for use of brain research in education and other contexts (OECD, 2002, p. 111). Pithy yet empty promises, such as learn vocabulary while you sleep or Brain Gym exercises make you smarter, are becoming more and more common in everyday life. Advertised by companies as “low-cost and easily implemented classroom approaches” (Howard-Jones, 2014, p. 819) promising to improve learning and/or memory performance, neuromyths find their way into teachers' methodological repertoires, which they pass on to their students with the best of intentions (Simmonds, 2014). These findings suggest that the prevalence of neuromyths should be responded to not only with criticism but also by constructively addressing the problem. Previous studies on neuromyths (cf. Figure 1) all indicate that teachers and university students are highly interested in neuroscience but need help to correctly relate elements of knowledge from cognitive psychology, neuroscience, and subject didactics and to critically engage with information lacking a grounding in proper evidence. Grospietsch and Mayer (2018) developed a neurodidactic concept for times in which the catchphrase brain-friendly learning enjoys persistent popularity to clarify for (pre-service) teachers and university educators that although neurodidactics is certainly a plausible concept, it can become a myth when applied incorrectly. Moreover, Papadatou-Pastou et al. (2017) emphasize the importance of developing an understanding among (pre-service) teachers of how neuroscience research is conducted and presented (e. g., understanding images which show increased brain activity). Consequently, both neuroscience content/methods and an accurate foundation for neurodidactics approaches should be incorporated into teacher education and university didactics. A corresponding curriculum would be quite helpful.
One aspect that university instructors need to be aware of when addressing neuromyths is that these misconceptions can be deeply biographically anchored and difficult to change (Grospietsch and Mayer, 2019). Grospietsch and Mayer (2018) show that students argue not only scientifically in support of neuromyths (e. g., based on neuroscience and cognitive psychology) but also use biographical arguments (e. g., referring to personal experiences) and that refutation can even reinforce their misconceptions (= backfire effect; cf. Cook and Lewandowsky, 2011). Work by Pettito and Dunbar (2004) has highlighted that students can stubbornly cling to their original beliefs despite empirical demonstrations and theoretical representations. Newton and Miah (2017) demonstrate this specifically for the neuromyth concerning the existence of learning styles. Moreover, based on a study by Kim and Sankey (2017), it must be acknowledged that pre-service teachers may have already learned neuromyths before beginning their university studies, that is, during their own school years. They can also be deeply convinced of their misconceptions due to their practical experiences or intuitively believe them to be true (cf. Blanchette Sarrasin et al., 2019). These are all potential reasons why few effective intervention approaches to combat neuromyths currently exist (Grospietsch and Mayer, 2018; McCarthy and Frantz, 2016; McMahon et al., 2019). Teaching strategies and methods that take up students' misconceptions, that deliberately bring them into a cognitive conflict, and that systematically expand them in the direction of scientifically appropriate conceptions have proven to be particularly effective at combating neuromyths (Grospietsch and Mayer, 2018). Based on this conceptual change theory (Vosniadou, 2013), a course was developed through an interdisciplinary collaboration at the University of Kassel (Grospietsch and Mayer, 2018). In contrast to merely imparting professional knowledge from cognitive psychology, neuroscience, and subject didactics, the instructional material used in this seminar, conceptual change texts, proved to be an effective and evidence-based means of translating neuroscientific content into the language of teachers. Moreover, both neuromyths and scientifically appropriate conceptions were sustainably professionalized with medium to large effect sizes (Grospietsch and Mayer, 2018). Based on our experiences, we can report that the quality of such learning programs can be improved by having neuroscientists and cognitive psychologists clear up neuromyths in a scientifically accurate way based on the most current research results. Precisely because disciplines have their unique methods and languages that are difficult to understand for experts in other areas, there is a need for cooperation among teacher educators, cognitive psychologists, and neuroscientists. Only by intensifying (existing) exchange networks can (pre-service) teachers' and university instructors' neuroscience literacy be improved and neuromyths related to brain-friendly learning be eliminated. In particular, neuroscientists interested in a content-based collaboration are warmly invited to contact us.
Funding source: Bundesministerium für Bildung und Forschung
Award Identifier / Grant number: 01JA1805
Award Identifier / Grant number: 01JA1505
About the authors

Finja Grospietsch studied biology and German education for secondary schools at Kiel University (M. Ed. 2015) and obtained her PhD in biology didactics (Dr. rer. Nat.) at the University of Kassel in 2020. She works there as a research associate and coordinator for the project PRONET2 (Professionalisierung durch Vernetzung - Fortführung und Potenzierung), which is part of the Qualitätsoffensive Lehrerbildung, a joint initiative of the German Federal Government and the Federal States.

Jürgen Mayer researches in the field of biology didactics at the University of Kassel. He studied biology, chemistry, philosophy and education at the universities in Göttingen and Kiel and obtained his PhD in biology at Kiel University in 1992. Following several years of research in biology didactics at the Leibniz Institute for Science and Mathematics Education at Kiel University, he assumed a professorship for biology didactics at Giessen University in 1999. In 2009, he transferred to a position as Professor of Biology Didactics at the University of Kassel. His current research focuses on teaching and learning research with respect to subject didactics, modeling and assessment of scientific competence and science teacher education.
Acknowledgments
We express special thanks to the working groups led by Prof. Dr. Tobias Richter (cognitive psychology) and Dr. Christine Nowack (human biology) for their support in constructing our biology didactics course “Brain-based Learning – Concept or Myth?” as well as the students who attended the course and contributed to its further development through their feedback. We also thank everyone who contributed to the success of our studies.
-
Funding: This project is part of the “Qualitätsoffensive Lehrerbildung”, a joint initiative of the German federal government and the federal states aiming to improve the quality of teacher training. The program was funded by the Federal Ministry of Education and Research under funding codes 01JA1505 and 01JA1805. The authors are responsible for the content of this publication.
References
Bartoszeck, A.B. and Bartoszeck, F.K. (2012). How in-service teachers perceive neuroscience as connected to education: An exploratory study. Eur. J. Educ. Res. 1, 301–319.10.12973/eu-jer.1.4.301Search in Google Scholar
Bear, M.F., Connors, B.W., and Paradiso, M.A. (2018). Neurowissenschaften: Ein grundlegendes Lehrbuch für Biologie, Medizin und Psychologie (Berlin: Springer).10.1007/978-3-662-57263-4Search in Google Scholar
Bellert, A. and Graham, L. (2013). Neuromyths and neurofacts: Information from cognitive neuroscience for classroom and learning support teachers. Spec. Educ. Perspect. 22, 7–20.Search in Google Scholar
Biswal, B.B., Mennes, M., Zuo, X.-N., Gohel, S., Kelly, C., Smith, S.M., Beckmann, C.F., Adelstein, J.S., Buckner, R.L., Colcombe, S., et al. (2010). Toward discovery science of human brain function. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 107, 4734–4739.10.1073/pnas.0911855107Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central
Blanchette Sarrasin, J., Riopel, M., and Masson, S. (2019). Neuromyths and their origin among teachers in Quebec. Mind Brain Educ. 13, 100–109.10.1111/mbe.12193Search in Google Scholar
Canbulat, T. and Kiriktas, H. (2017). Assessment of educational neuromyths among teachers and teacher candidates. JEL 6, 326.10.5539/jel.v6n2p326Search in Google Scholar
Carter, R. (2014). Das Gehirn: [Anatomie, Sinneswahrnehmung, Gedächtnis, Bewusstsein, Störungen] (München: Dorling Kindersley).Search in Google Scholar
Cook, J. and Lewandowsky, S. (2011). The Debunking Handbook (St. Lucia, Australien: University of Queensland).Search in Google Scholar
de Lussanet, M.H.E. and Osse, J.W.M. (2012). An ancestral axial twist explains the contralateral forebrain and the optic chiasm in vertebrates. Anim. Biol. 62, 193–216.10.1163/157075611X617102Search in Google Scholar
Dekker, S., Lee, N.C., Howard-Jones, P., and Jolles, J. (2012). Neuromyths in education: Prevalence and predictors of misconceptions among teachers. Front. Psychol. 3, 429.10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00429Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central
Deligiannidi, K. and Howard-Jones, P.A. (2015). The neuroscience literacy of teachers in Greece. Proc. Soc. Behav. 174, 3909–3915.10.1016/j.sbspro.2015.01.1133Search in Google Scholar
Dündar, S. and Gündüz, N. (2016). Misconceptions regarding the brain: The neuromyths of preservice teachers. Mind Brain Educ. 10, 212–232.10.1111/mbe.12119Search in Google Scholar
Düvel, N., Wolf, A., and Kopiez, R. (2017). Neuromyths in music education: Prevalence and predictors of misconceptions among teachers and students. Front. Psychol. 8, 629.10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00629Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central
Ferrero, M., Garaizar, P., and Vadillo, M.A. (2016). Neuromyths in education: Prevalence among Spanish teachers and an exploration of cross-cultural variation. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 10, 496.10.3389/fnhum.2016.00496Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central
Folta-Schoofs, K. and Ostermann, B. (2019). Neurodidaktik: Grundlagen für Studium und Praxis (Stuttgart: Verlag W. Kohlhammer).Search in Google Scholar
Fuentes, A. and Risso, A. (2015). Evaluación de conocimientos y actitudes sobre neuromitos en futuros/as maestros/as. Rev. Estud. Invest. Psicol. Educ. 6, 193–198.10.17979/reipe.2015.0.06.530Search in Google Scholar
Gais, S. and Born, J. (2004). Declarative memory consolidation: Mechanisms acting during human sleep. Learn. Mem. 11, 679–685.10.1101/lm.80504Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central
Gleichgerrcht, E., Lira Luttges, B., Salvarezza, F., and Campos, A.L. (2015). Educational neuromyths among teachers in Latin America. Mind Brain Educ. 9, 170–178.10.1111/mbe.12086Search in Google Scholar
Grospietsch, F. and Mayer, J. (2018). Professionalizing pre-service biology teachers' misconceptions about learning and the brain through conceptual change. Educ. Sci. 8, 120.10.3390/educsci8030120Search in Google Scholar
Grospietsch, F. and Mayer, J. (2019). Pre-service science teachers' neuroscience literacy: Neuromyths and a professional understanding of learning and memory. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 13, 20.10.3389/fnhum.2019.00020Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central
Häusel, H.-G. (ed.). (2008). Neuromarketing: Erkenntnisse der Hirnforschung für Markenführung, Werbung und Verkauf (Freiburg: Haufe).Search in Google Scholar
Hermida, M.J., Segretin, M.S., Soni García, A., and Lipina, S.J. (2016). Conceptions and misconceptions about neuroscience in preschool teachers: A study from Argentina. Educ. Res. 58, 457–472.10.1080/00131881.2016.1238585Search in Google Scholar
Herreros, C. (2012). #Neuro-management (Madrid: LID Editorial Empresarial).Search in Google Scholar
Herrmann, U. (ed.). (2009). Neurodidaktik: Grundlagen und Vorschläge für gehirngerechtes Lehren und Lernen (Weinheim Basel: Beltz).Search in Google Scholar
Höffler, T.N., Koć-Januchta, M., and Leutner, D. (2017). More evidence for three types of cognitive style: Validating the object-spatial imagery and verbal questionnaire using eye tracking when learning with texts and pictures: Evidence for three types of cognitive style. Appl. Cogn. Psychol. 31, 109–115.10.1002/acp.3300Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central
Horvath, J.C., Donoghue, G.M., Horton, A.J., Lodge, J.M., and Hattie, J.A.C. (2018). On the irrelevance of neuromyths to teacher effectiveness: Comparing neuro-literacy levels amongst award-winning and non-award winning teachers. Front. Psychol. 9, 1666.10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01666Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central
Howard-Jones, P.A. (2010). Introducing neuroeducational research – Neuroscience, education and the brain from contexts to practice (London; New York: Routledge).10.4324/9780203867303Search in Google Scholar
Howard-Jones, P.A. (2014). Neuroscience and education: Myths and messages. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 15, 817–824.10.1038/nrn3817Search in Google Scholar PubMed
Howard-Jones, P.A., Franey, L., Mashmoushi, R., and Liao, Y.-C. (2009). The neuroscience literacy of trainee teachers. Paper presented at British Educational Research Association Annual Conference, Manchester.Search in Google Scholar
Im, S., Cho, J.-Y., Dubinsky, J.M., and Varma, S. (2018). Taking an educational psychology course improves neuroscience literacy but does not reduce belief in neuromyths. PLoS One 13, e0192163.10.1371/journal.pone.0192163Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central
Jäncke, L. (2013). Lehrbuch kognitive Neurowissenschaften (Milwaukee, WI: Hans Huber).Search in Google Scholar
Karakus, O., Howard-Jones, P.A., and Jay, T. (2015). Primary and secondary school teachers' knowledge and misconceptions about the brain in Turkey. Proc. Soc. Behav. 174, 1933–1940.10.1016/j.sbspro.2015.01.858Search in Google Scholar
Kim, M. and Sankey, D. (2017). Philosophy, neuroscience and pre-service teachers' beliefs in neuromyths: A call for remedial action. Educ. Philos. Theory 50, 1–14.10.1080/00131857.2017.1395736Search in Google Scholar
Krammer, G., Vogel, S.E., Yardimci, T., and Grabner, R.H. (2019). Neuromythen sind zu Beginn des Lehramtsstudiums prävalent und unabhängig vom Wissen über das menschliche Gehirn. Z Bildungsforsch. 9, 221–246.10.1007/s35834-019-00238-2Search in Google Scholar
Langfeldt, H.-P. and Nieder, T. (2004). Subjektive Lerntheorien von Lehramtsstudierenden – ein Forschungsprogramm zur Qualitätsverbesserung in der universitären Lehrerbildung. PISA und die Konsequenzen für die erziehungswissenschaftliche Forschung. D. Lenzen, and J. Baumert, eds. (Wiesbaden: VS, Verl. für Sozialwiss), pp. 159–170.10.1007/978-3-322-86897-8_9Search in Google Scholar
Lethaby, C. and Harries, P. (2016). Learning styles and teacher training: Are we perpetuating neuromyths? ELT J. 70, 16–27.10.1093/elt/ccv051Search in Google Scholar
Macdonald, K., Germine, L., Anderson, A., Christodoulou, J., and McGrath, L.M. (2017). Dispelling the myth: Training in education or neuroscience decreases but does not eliminate beliefs in neuromyths. Front. Psychol. 8, 1314.10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01314Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central
McCarthy, M.A. and Frantz, S. (2016). Challenging the status quo: Evidence that introductory psychology can dispel myths. Teach. Psychol. 43, 211–214.10.1177/0098628316649470Search in Google Scholar
McMahon, K., Yeh, C.S., and Etchells, P.J. (2019). The impact of a modified initial teacher education on challenging trainees' understanding of neuromyths. Mind Brain Educ. 13, 288–297.10.1111/mbe.12219Search in Google Scholar
Metzger, C. (2018). Neuroarchitektur (Berlin: Jovis).Search in Google Scholar
Newton, P.M. and Miah, M. (2017). Evidence-based higher education – Is the learning styles ‘myth’ important? Front. Psychol. 8, 444.10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00444Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central
Organisation for Economic Co-operation and Development [OECD] (2002). Understanding the brain: Towards a new learning science. (Paris: OECD).Search in Google Scholar
Papadatou-Pastou, M., Haliou, E., and Vlachos, F. (2017). Brain knowledge and the prevalence of neuromyths among prospective teachers in Greece. Front. Psychol. 8, 804.10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00804Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central
Pasquinelli, E. (2012). Neuromyths: Why do they exist and persist? Mind Brain Educ. 6, 89–96.10.1111/j.1751-228X.2012.01141.xSearch in Google Scholar
Pei, X., Howard-Jones, P.A., Zhang, S., Liu, X., and Jin, Y. (2015). Teachers' understanding about the brain in East China. Proc. Soc. Behav. 174, 3681–3688.10.1016/j.sbspro.2015.01.1091Search in Google Scholar
Petitto, L.-A. and Dunbar, K. (2004). New findings from educational neuroscience on bilingual brains, scientific brains, and the educated mind. Building Usable Knowledge in Mind, Brain, & Education. K., Fischer and T., Katzir, eds. (Cambridge, MA, USA: Cambridge University Press), pp. 1–20.Search in Google Scholar
Rato, J.R., Abreu, A.M., and Castro-Caldas, A. (2013). Neuromyths in education: What is fact and what is fiction for Portuguese teachers? Educ. Res. 55, 441–453.10.1080/00131881.2013.844947Search in Google Scholar
Ruhaak, A.E. and Cook, B.G. (2018). The prevalence of educational neuromyths among pre‐service special education teachers. Mind Brain Educ. 12, mbe.12181.10.1111/mbe.12181Search in Google Scholar
Simmonds, A. (2014). How Neuroscience is Affecting Education: Report of Teacher and Parent Survey. (London: Wellcome Trust).Search in Google Scholar
Tardif, E., Doudin, P.-A., and Meylan, N. (2015). Neuromyths among teachers and student teachers: Neuromyths. Mind Brain Educ. 9, 50–59.10.1111/mbe.12070Search in Google Scholar
van Dijk, W. and Lane, H.B. (2018). The brain and the US education system: Perpetuation of neuromyths. Exceptionality 1–14.10.1080/09362835.2018.1480954Search in Google Scholar
Vosniadou, S., ed. (2013). International Handbook of Research on Conceptual Change (New York, London: Routledge/Taylor & Francis Group).10.4324/9780203154472Search in Google Scholar
Zhang, R., Jiang, Y., Dang, B., and Zhou, A. (2019). Neuromyths in Chinese classrooms: Evidence from headmasters in an underdeveloped region of China. Front. Educ. 4, 8.10.3389/feduc.2019.00008Search in Google Scholar
Supplementary Material
A German translation of this review can be found online at: https://doi.org/10.1515/nf-2020-0006.
© 2020 Walter de Gruyter GmbH, Berlin/Boston