Abstract
The current study experimentally examined whether labial consonants were sound-symbolically associated with the images of softness and cuteness in Japanese and English. The results showed that all the bilabial consonants [p, b, m, ɸ, w] used in Japanese convey such images. In English, the consonants evoking the image of softness were bilabials but not labiodentals, and those linked to the image of cuteness were unaspirated, low-frequency bilabials. These results demonstrated the pluripotentiality of sound symbolism, meaning that a certain set of linguistic sounds evokes different meanings and images both within a single language and across languages (e.g. Winter et al. 2019; Kawahara and Kumagai to appear). Moreover, under the backcloth that the description of the glide /w/ concerning the place of articulation in Japanese and English is not uncontroversial, the current paper indicates – based on the current sound-symbolic experimental results – that the glide /w/ is phonologically labial in each language.
1 Introduction
1.1 Sound symbolism of bilabial consonants
In linguistics, it is assumed that an arbitrary relationship exists between sounds and meanings (de Saussure 1916). This enables people to refer to a certain item as one name in one language but to the same item as a different name in another language; for example, the animal that is referred to as cat in English is referred to as neko in Japanese and gato in Spanish. Natural languages also showcase precisely the opposite characterization – that a linguistic sound or feature evokes particular meanings and images, which is referred to as sound symbolism (Hinton et al. 1994/2006). A typical example of sound symbolism is the openness of vowels in articulation being sound-symbolically linked with the image of largeness (Newman 1933; Sapir 1929). Subsequent studies have confirmed that the sound-symbolic effect holds for numerous languages (e.g. Ultan 1978; Berlin 2006; Shinohara and Kawahara 2016). Sound symbolism has now become one of the featured research themes in linguistics, psychology, and cognitive science (for overview papers, see, e.g. Hinton et al. 1994/2006; Nuckolls 1999; Akita 2015; Lockwood and Dingemanse 2015; Sidhu and Pexman 2018; Kawahara 2020). These overview articles and edited books on sound symbolism show that a variety of research topics have been explored – such as basic vocabulary and interjections (e.g. Wichmann et al. 2010; Urban 2011; Blasi et al. 2016; Winter et al. 2019), human names (e.g. Cutler et al. 1990; Wright et al. 2005; Pitcher et al. 2013; Shinohara and Kawahara 2013; Kawahara et al. 2015; Kawahara 2017; Sidhu et al. 2019), fictional names of characters in TV animation and computer games (e.g., Pokémon character names by Kawahara et al. (2018) et seq.; Kumagai et al. (2020); Sidhu and Pexman (2019); Uno et al. (2020)), and brand names (e.g. Peterson and Ross 1972; Klink 2000; Yorkston and Menon 2004; Lowrey and Shrum 2007; Pogacar et al. 2015; Bolts et al. 2016).
A number of studies are available on the sound-symbolic effects of bilabial consonants, the sounds produced by touching the upper and lower lips together. In psychology, D’Onofrio (2014) used the maluma-takete paradigm (Köhler 1929) to examine whether phonetic features affect the selection of shapes; the results suggested that labial consonants more likely match with the round shapes than with the spiky ones. Apart from such visual–auditory, sound-symbolic associations, recent studies have clarified that, even within a single language, bilabial consonants can be associated with multiple meanings and images such as babyness, cuteness, softness, innocence, and smallness. Kumagai and Kawahara (2017, 2020) suggest that Japanese speakers associate bilabial consonants [p, b, m, ɸ, w] with the image of babies.[1] Most baby diaper brands in Japan contain the bilabial consonants [p, m] (e.g. muunii, meriizu, mamiipoko) (Kawahara 2017). This observation is interesting because bilabial consonants tend to appear in babbling in the earlier stages of language acquisition (Jakobson 1941/1968; MacNeilage et al. 1994). Kumagai and Kawahara (2017, 2020) conducted a forced-choice test that examined whether words that contain bilabial consonants (e.g. paraperu) are more appropriate for baby diaper brands than those that do not contain any bilabial consonants (e.g. tarateru). The study found that names that contain bilabial consonants are preferred as a baby diaper brand name. Kumagai and Kawahara (2017, 2020) also conducted a free elicitation task that asked native speakers of Japanese to come up with brand names for both baby diaper brands and adult cosmetics. The results showed that bilabial consonants were more likely to appear in baby diaper brand names than in cosmetics brand names.
Kumagai and Kawahara’s study has inspired researchers to analyse sound-symbolic effects of bilabial consonants; consequently, intriguing findings have been reported by several studies. Kumagai (2019) indicated a new type of alternation in which [h] becomes [p] in girls’ nicknames in Japanese (e.g. Haruka → Paruru, an ex member of AKB48, a young Japanese girl’s idol group). Since the new type occurs even in the word-initial position, it should be distinguished from the traditional [h] → [p] alternation observed word internally in Sino-Japanese words (e.g. kin “gold” + hatsu “hair” → kin-patsu “gold hair”). He also demonstrated via experiments that the bilabial [p] was the consonant that is most likely ([m] being the second most likely) to express cuteness.[2] Kawahara (2019) investigated the names of cute girls’ fighters who appeared in a Japanese TV animation named PreCure, in which normal girls transformed themselves into cute fighters called “PreCure” with new names that tended to possess bilabial consonants (e.g. Cure Peach, Cure Milky). Further, experiments using imaginary Pokémon characters demonstrated that, for Japanese speakers, names with bilabial consonants were more appropriate for fairy types, which look cuter than normal character types in Japanese (Kawahara and Kumagai 2019b) and that for English speakers, names with the bilabial [p] were also more appropriate for fairy-type characters (Kawahara et al. 2020a).[3] Kumagai et al. (to appear) examined images of hardness and softness in the texture of snacks, thereby experimentally demonstrating that, among Japanese voiceless plosives [p, b, t, d, k, ɡ], the bilabial [p] is more likely to evoke the image of softness than non-bilabial sounds like [k, ɡ] (for similar results, see also Akita & McLean to appear). Hosokawa et al. (2018) and Uno et al. (2020) found that bilabial consonants were more likely to occur in the English names of non-villains that appear in Disney’s animated films (e.g. Pinocchio, Mickey) than in those of villains. Further, the number of bilabial consonants are negatively correlated with size in Japanese Pokémon names (Shih et al. 2018, 2019), and names that contain bilabial consonants are less appropriate for post-evolution Pokémon characters than those that contain coronal consonants in experiments using imaginary Pokémon characters (Kawahara and Moore to appear). To summarize, what has been clarified in previous studies is that it is likely that [p] is associated with the image of cuteness in both Japanese and English and that bilabial consonants can evoke an image of innocence and smallness in English.
1.2 Labiality of [w]: Evidence from sound symbolism
As evident from the above discussion, although there has been an increased interest in sound-symbolic effects of bilabial consonants, a few studies utilize evidence from sound-symbolic studies to phonological argumentation (Kumagai and Kawahara 2017, 2020; Kawahara 2019; Kawahara and Kumagai 2019a, 2019b; see Kawahara 2020 for sound symbolism in phonology). In line with these studies, the current study addresses the issue of the place of articulation of glide /w/, based on sound-symbolic evidence. According to the International Phonetic Association, [w] is described as a labiovelar glide, pronounced with upper and lower lips touching together and with the back of the tongue moving upward to the velum. Since it involves the lips, it may not be a problem that [w] is included in the bilabial consonant group. However, there is no unanimous description of the glide concerning the place of articulation in introductory textbooks and papers on Japanese and English: the Japanese [w] is described as a labial (Shibatani 1990; Kubozono 2015), as a velar (Yamaguchi 2007; Tsujimura 2014; Pintér 2015), or as a labiovelar (Labrune 2012);[4] the English glide /w/ is described as a labiovelar (or labial-velar) glide or approximant (Giegerich 1992; Hammond 1999; Cruttenden 2014; Yavas 2016; Collins et al. 2019; Carr 2020) or as a bilabial approximant (Roach 2009).
A number of scholars have addressed the issue of the glide /w/ concerning the place of articulation (e.g. Anderson 1976; Ohala and Lorentz 1977; Gick 2003; Nevins and Chitoran 2008), showing that the phonological behaviour of the glide /w/ varies across languages.[5] Then is there evidence to determine the place-of-articulation feature of the glide /w/ in Japanese and English? Kumagai (2017) examined whether the Obligatory Contour Principle (OCP)-labial effect[6] is generalized in a Japanese morphophonological phenomenon known as rendaku (see Vance 1987, 2015; Vance and Irwin 2016 for rendaku), suggesting that the glide /w/ is not specified with the place-of-articulation feature [labial] in phonology; this is because, unlike the other labial consonants /p, b, m, ɸ/, it does not participate in the OCP-labial effect. Meanwhile, it is observed that an English-speaking child replaces the cluster /sm, sw, sp/ with the labiodental [f] (e.g. smell = [f]ell; Smith = [f]ith; spoon = [f]oon; Tessier 2016: 176), which can be explained, provided that bilabials /m, w, p/ share the place-of-articulation feature [labial] with labiodental [f] and that the labiality is faithfully preserved between underlying and surface representations (i.e. input and output in optimality-theoretic terms; for Optimality Theory, see Prince and Smolensky 1993/2004; McCarthy 2002, 2008).[7] In addition, as evident below, a number of sound-symbolic studies have addressed the issue. Kawahara and Kumagai (2019) showed that bilabial consonants, except for [w], were appropriate for the Pokémon names of fairy characters. Kawahara’s (2019) analysis of PreCure names showed that the glide /w/ was less likely to appear in their names than other bilabial consonants. Since the glide /w/ shows different distributions from other bilabial consonants, these two studies may become examples casting doubts on the labiality of the glide /w/. Meanwhile, Kumagai and Kawahara (2017, 2020) showed that all the bilabial consonants /p, b, m, ɸ, w/ are appropriate for baby diaper brand names, suggesting that the glide /w/ should be categorized into the [labial] group. To summarize, not all sound-symbolic studies reach a consensus regarding the labiality of the Japanese glide /w/, and, to the best of my knowledge, there are very few discussions based on sound-symbolic evidence in English.[8]
1.3 Purposes of the current study
The first purpose of the current study is to experimentally examine whether labial consonants are sound-symbolically associated with the images of softness and cuteness in Japanese and English (yawarakai “soft” and kawaii “cute” in Japanese)[9] for the following reasons. First, it remains unclear whether all bilabial consonants are associated with the image of softness in Japanese. The bilabial [p] was the most likely consonant to evoke the image of softness in Japanese, as lips are softer than other articulatory organs such as the alveolar ridge and the velum in the oral cavity (Kumagai et al. to appear). If the sound-symbolic association of bilabial [p] results from the sensation that lips are tied with an image of softness, it is predicted that all bilabial consonants are associated with the image of softness not only in a single language but also in others, such as English.[10] Second, although bilabial [p, m] are likely to express cuteness in Japanese (Kumagai 2019), it is underexplored whether all bilabial consonants are associated with the image of cuteness in that language. Where does the image of cuteness originate from? One possibility is that it results from a (cute) pouting gesture that appears when humans articulate bilabial consonants. The pouting gesture is known as part of the photographic pose called a duck face. A study on duck faces for Chinese speakers (Qiu et al. 2015) showed that people making duck faces are more likely to be neurotic; however, on the other hand, online articles showed that duck faces appear to be sexually enticing,[11] and that the Japanese duck face, called ahiru-guti “a duck mouth” or tyun-gao “face like a bird,” is also likely to be an appealing expression.[12] [13] If the pouting gesture evokes the image of cuteness, then it is not difficult to imagine that all bilabial consonants evoke such an image in Japanese, let alone other languages. Third, even if it is found that bilabial consonants are associated with the images of softness and cuteness in Japanese, it remains to be determined whether the sound-symbolic associations are conveyed by labial consonants, including labiodental consonants and bilabial consonants, because of the absence of any non-bilabial consonants such as labiodentals in the Japanese language. Relatedly, although the p-initial names were more appropriate for the fairy-type (cute) Pokémon characters (Kawahara et al. 2020a), it remains to be seen whether the sound-symbolic association involves aspiration, as the study conducted an experiment using only nonce names that begin with p, but not those that begin with sp (when the following vowel is stressed, the p-initial is usually aspirated but the sp-initial ([s] + consonant clusters) is unaspirated). Thus, in order to resolve these issues, the current study examines whether the English labiodental fricative [f] has the same sound-symbolic effect as other bilabial consonants and whether aspirated and unaspirated bilabial consonants sound soft and cute to English speakers, respectively.
The second purpose of the current study is, based on the current sound-symbolic experimental results for Japanese and English, the discussion on whether the place-of-articulation feature [labial] is specified for the glide /w/ in each language. If the experiment shows that the glide [w] – in addition to bilabial consonants such as [p, b, m] – evokes the images of softness and cuteness in Japanese and English, then it will be concluded that it shares the feature [labial] with other bilabial consonants. Meanwhile, if the glide does not show the same tendency as other bilabial consonants, then it is unlikely to possess the feature [labial]. The current article suggests that the glide /w/ is phonologically labial in Japanese and English, although the English results were less clear.
2 Experiment
2.1 Task and stimuli
The task of the current experiment was that, given nonce words, participants were asked which of them sounded cuter or softer (kawaii and yawarakai in Japanese, respectively).[14] The targeted languages were Japanese and English, each of which has different labial consonants and phonotactic restrictions, and thus different stimuli were prepared so as not to deviate from the phonotactic rules of each language. There were five to six conditions, each of which had three pairs of nonce words with and without labial consonants, as shown in Tables 1 and 2. The labial consonants in the experimental group were replaced with the alveolar counterparts.
Pairs of stimuli (Japanese)
Exp | Katakana | Phonetics forms | Cont | Katakana | Phonetics forms | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Cond 1 | [p] | パロポン | paropon | vs | [t] | タロトン | taroton |
ぺリパン | peripan | vs | テリタン | teritan | |||
ポレポン | porepon | vs | トレトン | toreton | |||
Cond 2 | [b] | バロボン | barobon | vs | [d] | ダロドン | darodon |
ベリバン | beriban | vs | デリダン | deridan | |||
ボレボン | borebon | vs | ドレドン | doredon | |||
Cond 3 | [m] | マロモン | maromon | vs | [n] | ナロノン | naronon |
メリマン | meriman | vs | ネリナン | nerinan | |||
モレモン | moremon | vs | ノレノン | norenon | |||
Cond 4 | [ɸ] | フロフン | ɸuroɸun | vs | [s] | スロスン | surosun |
フリフン | ɸuriɸun | vs | スリスン | surisun | |||
フレフン | ɸureɸun | vs | スレスン | suresun | |||
Cond 5 | [w] | ワロワン | warowan | vs | [j] | ヤロヤン | jarojan |
ワリワン | wariwan | vs | ヤリヤン | jarijan | |||
ワレワン | warewan | vs | ヤレヤン | jarejan |
Pairs of stimuli (English)
Exp | Alphabets | Cont | Alphabets | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Cond 1 | [ph] | paungpaung | vs | [th] | taungtaung |
piungpiung | vs | tiungtiung | |||
peangpeang | vs | teangteang | |||
Cond 2 | [p] | spaungspaung | vs | [t] | staungstaung |
spiungspiung | vs | stiungstiung | |||
speangspeang | vs | steangsteang | |||
Cond 3 | [b] | baungbaung | vs | [d] | daungdaung |
biungbiuing | vs | diungdiuing | |||
beangbeang | vs | deangdeang | |||
Cond 4 | [f] | faungfaung | vs | [s] | saungsaung |
fiungfiung | vs | siungsiung | |||
feangfeang | vs | seangseang | |||
Cond 5 | [m] | maungmaung | vs | [n] | naungnaung |
miungmiung | vs | niungniung | |||
meangmeang | vs | neangneang | |||
Cond 6 | [w] | waungwaung | vs | [j] | yaungyaung |
wiungwiung | vs | yiungyiung | |||
weangweang | vs | yeangyeang |
The Japanese language has five labial consonants: the voiceless bilabial plosive [p], the voiced bilabial plosive [b], the bilabial nasal [m], the voiceless bilabial fricative [ɸ], and the labiovelar glide (approximant) [w] (e.g. Labrune 2012). All these consonants can occur in the onset position. The first three [p, b, m] can precede any vowel [i, e, a, o, u]. There are a number of phonotactic restrictions on [ɸ] and [w]. Although the [ɸ] can precede any vowel, and the [w] can precede [i, e, a, u] in loanwords (e.g. Pintér 2015), [ɸ] is known as an allophone of /h/ before [u], and [w] is followed by [a] in native Japanese words (e.g. Tsujimura 2014). Thus, in the stimuli, the vowels after [ɸ] and [w] were restricted to [u, a], respectively. All the stimuli consisted of four morae, and the targeted consonants were placed at the onset position in the first and third morae.
The English language has five labial consonants: the voiceless bilabial plosive [p], the voiced bilabial plosive [b], the bilabial nasal [m], the voiceless labiodental fricative [f], and the labiovelar glide (approximant) [w]. The only difference between Japanese and English is that while the Japanese fricative is a bilabial [ɸ], the English counterpart is a labiodental [f]. The current experiment did not target the voiced labiodental fricative [v] because, as the Japanese language had no voiced fricative, such as [β], it was difficult to compare the results of both languages. It is well known that voiceless plosives exhibit different realizations according to phonetic environments: /p/ is phonetically realized as an aspirated consonant [ph] when it is placed in the onset position of a syllable and when the syllable to which it belongs is assigned stress. However, it is not produced with aspiration in [s] + consonant clusters (e.g. /sp/). The current experiment distinguished these two different environments and set up a total of six conditions [ph, p, b, m, f, w]. All the stimuli consisted of two syllables, and the targeted consonants were syllable-initial in each of the stimuli.
All the pairs were randomized per participant and per session. Following the previous experimental studies on bilabial consonants, as detailed in Section 1.1 (Kumagai and Kawahara 2017, 2020; Kawahara and Kumagai 2019a, 2019b; Kumagai 2019; Kawahara and Moore to appear; Kumagai et al. to appear), the current study adopted experiments using non-auditory stimuli; the stimuli were provided in katakana orthography for Japanese speakers and in the Latin alphabet for English speakers.
2.2 Procedure
The present study used an online questionnaire through SurveyMonkey. The experiment consisted of two sessions: in the first session, participants were first provided an explanation that they would be presented with two names per question and that their task was to choose which they felt was a cuter name (kawaii namae “cute name” in Japanese); thereafter, they were asked to judge all of the pairs of the stimuli. As soon as the first session ended, the second session began, where the participants were provided an explanation that they would be presented with two names per question and that their task was to choose which name they felt had an image of “soft” (yawarakai imeeji “soft image” in Japanese); thereafter, they were asked to judge all the pairs of the stimuli. After the second session, the participants were asked several questions regarding their age, gender, and whether they were native speakers of Japanese/English. Moreover, the participants in the Japanese experiment were asked whether they had studied sound symbolism, and participants in the English experiment were asked whether they were familiar with the term sound symbolism.[15]
2.3 Participants
For the Japanese participants, the author of the current article advertised the experiment in university classes, and a total of 83 native-speaking students participated. They did not belong to the university’s linguistics department or related fields, and they reported having never studied sound symbolism. One of the participants did not complete the task, and, thus, data for the remaining 82 were analysed. All the participants were female and their age range was 18–30 years.
For the English participants, the weblink for the experiment was distributed through SurveyMonkey, and a total of 106 native speakers were recruited. The survey asked whether they were familiar with the term sound symbolism and 60 participants answered in the affirmative; thus, they were excluded from the analysis, and the remaining 46 speakers were targeted. There was a total of 36 female speakers; 17 of the speakers were aged under 30 years.
2.4 Results
The current section presents the results of each experiment. The figures display the rate at which the nonce words containing labial consonants were chosen. Error bars represent 95% confident intervals.
2.4.1 Softness in Japanese
Figure 1 presents the rate at which Japanese speakers judged the stimuli with bilabial consonants to be soft: [p] = 0.614, [b] = 0.711, [m] = 0.695, [ɸ] (represented as f) = 0.911, [w] = 0.797. All the bilabial consonants were above chance level. Following the previous studies that conducted a forced-choice task (e.g. Kumagai and Kawahara 2017, 2020), the author made a generalized linear mixed-effects logistic regression (glmer) analysis, with participants and items being the random effects. Consequently, the five consonants were significantly judged as soft names (β = 2.471, SE = 0.323, z = 7.646, p < 0.001). In order to compare the number of observed outcomes to that of expected outcomes, the author also ran binomial tests; the results revealed that all the five consonants showed the significance of the observed outcomes ([p] = 155/246 (= 82 participants × 3 pairs), p < 0.001; [b] = 175/246, p < 0.001; [m] = 171/246, p < 0.001; [ɸ] = 224/246, p < 0.001; [w] = 196/246, p < 0.001).

Rates of selecting names with labial consonants for a “soft” name (Japanese).
2.4.2 Cuteness in Japanese
Figure 2 depicts the results for the image of cuteness in Japanese. The rates of each response were [p] = 0.724, [b] = 0.707, [m] = 0.642, [ɸ] (represented as f) = 0.776, and [w] = 0.756. Similar to the results of softness, all the bilabial consonants were above chance level. A glmer analysis showed that these consonants were significantly judged as cute names (β = 2.036, SE = 0.228, z = 8.917, p < 0.001). Moreover, a binomial test showed significance in the observed outcomes of all consonants ([p] = 178/246, p < 0.001; [b] = 174/246, p < 0.001; [m] = 158/246, p < 0.001; [ɸ] = 191/246, p < 0.001; [w] = 186/246, p < 0.001).

Rates of selecting names with labial consonants for a “cute” name (Japanese).
2.4.3 Softness in English
Figure 3 depicts the extent to which English speakers judged the stimuli containing labial consonants as a soft name. The following rates are presented in each graph: [ph] (represented as ph) = 0.754, [p] (represented as sp) = 0.746, [b] = 0.775, [f] = 0.384, [m] = 0.703, [w] = 0.601. All the bar graphs except the fourth one were above chance level. A binomial test indicated the significance of the expected results for [ph, p, b, m, w] ([ph] = 104/138 (= 46 participants × 3 pairs), p < 0.001; [p] = 103/138, p < 0.001; [b] = 107/138, p < 0.001; [m] = 97/138, p < 0.001; [w] = 83/138, p < 0.05) but showed significance of the unexpected result for [f] ([f] = 53/138, p < 0.01). This implies that it is evident that the labiodental [f] cannot evoke the image of softness. Then the author conducted a glmer analysis that excluded the data of responses to the labiodental [f]; the results revealed that the names that contain bilabial consonants [ph, p, b, m, w] were judged to be soft names (β = 1.188, SE = 0.151, z = 7.844, p < 0.001).

Rates of selecting names with labial consonants for a “soft” name (English).
2.4.4 Cuteness in English
Figure 4 depicts the responses by English speakers in which the stimuli with labial consonants sounded cuter than those without them: [ph] (represented as ph) = 0.523, [p] (represented as sp) = 0.616, [b] = 0.775, [f] = 0.196, [m] = 0.659, and [w] = 0.37. The expected responses were obtained in the second, third, and fifth graphs (i.e. [p, b, m]) but not in the others (i.e. [ph, f, w]). A binomial test yielded significant results for [p, b, m] ([p] = 85/138, p < 0.01 (= 46 participants × 3 pairs); [b] = 107/138, p < 0.001; [m] = 91/138, p < 0.001) but showed no significant result for [ph] ([ph] = 72/138, p = 0.671) and significance of the unexpected result for [f, w] ([f] = 27/138, p < 0.001; [w] = 51/138 p < 0.01). Then a glmer analysis with the three consonants [ph, f, w] excluded indicated that the responses to three consonants [p, b, m] were significant (β = 1.553, SE = 0.173, z = 8.952, p < 0.001).

Rates of selecting names with labial consonants for a “cute” name (English).
3 Discussion
3.1 The image of softness
The results of the current experiment showed that the names that contain bilabial consonants were judged to be cuter names (kawaii namae in Japanese) than those that do not contain any bilabial consonants. This suggests that all the bilabial consonants used in Japanese ([p, b, m, ɸ, w]) were sound-symbolically associated with the image of softness. There are two possible explanations for these results. The first one is the physiological idea that softness results from the texture of the lips (Kumagai et al. to appear). The second explanation involves acoustic motivation. Ohala (1984, 1994) proposed the Frequency Code Hypothesis in which “low-frequency consonants are associated with low-frequency sounds, large size, softness, and heavy, slow movements” (Hinton Nichols and Ohala 1994/2006: 10). If bilabial consonants are assumed to have low-frequency energy (Berlin 2006), it is possible that Japanese speakers associate them with the image of softness.
However, the situation is different in English. The results showed that the consonants judged to be soft were [ph, p, b, m, w] but not [f]. The labiodental [f] does not evoke the image of softness, as it involves only the lower lip and not both lips, which may not satisfy the condition that lips are softer than other articulatory organs in the oral cavity (Kumagai et al. to appear). Moreover, since both [ph] and [p] exhibited significant results for the image of softness, aspiration was not significant in the relationship between bilabial consonants and the image of softness, at least, in English.
The current results pertaining to the labiodental [f] require further discussion. The binomial test yielded the unexpected result that the response to [s] in the control group was significant, which implies that the alveolar [s] was more likely to be judged to be soft than the labiodental [f]. However, only from these results, it may be difficult to conclude that the alveolar [s] can evoke the image of softness. The reason for these results requires analysis with new experiments in further research.
3.2 The image of cuteness
The current results indicated that, as was the case with the image of softness, all the bilabial consonants were associated with the image of cuteness in Japanese, which also replicated Kumagai’s (2019) results of the image of cuteness. As mentioned in Section 1.3, one possibility for the origin of the image of cuteness is that it resulted from a pouting gesture produced by articulating bilabial consonants. Another possibility is that a sound-symbolic association resulted from another in the minds of language speakers. Previous studies have already shown that a meaning or image is mutually linked to another (see also French 1977; Gallace et al. 2011; Kawahara and Kumagai to appear). Thus, it is possible that the image of cuteness may be mutually associated with that of softness in Japanese. The sound-symbolic association between bilabials and the image of softness could have produced the sound-symbolic association between bilabials and the image of cuteness. The opposite case is also possible: the sound-symbolic association of cuteness may have produced that of softness.
The results for the image of cuteness in English were more complex: bilabial [p, b, m] were judged to be cute, while [ph, f, w] were not. Unlike the case of Japanese, the account only with the pouting gesture is insufficient. Then are there any additional possible reasons for these results? First, the English word cute is not semantically equal to the Japanese word kawaii, which may be responsible for the less clear results in English. The English word cute means not only “attractive” but also “clever/cunning,” which may have been reflected in the current results (see also Section 3.3). For example, although voiced obstruents evoke a negative image due to their aerodynamic challenge (Uno et al. 2020), if low-frequency consonants [b, m], including voiced obstruents, can evoke the image of villainess, then it is possible that the names that contain the low-frequency consonants [b, m] will be more likely judged to be cuter names (including the meaning of cleverness/cunningness).[16] This may account for why [b, m] is higher than ph and sp [ph, p] in terms of the rate presented in Figure 4.
Second, the names with [ph] were not significantly judged to be cute, which was not consistent with the results of Kawahara et al. (2020a) that the p-initial name was appropriate for the (cute) fairy type of Pokémon characters. Where did the discrepancy come from? It may be difficult to draw a conclusion at the moment because the experimental designs of each study were not completely equal, but, as the p-initial name usually involves aspiration, it is necessary to conduct an experiment with auditory stimuli, as there is a possibility that aspiration (with strong expelled breath) can evoke the image of strength, which may not have matched with the image of cuteness.
Third, the results for [w] are rather puzzling, because it belongs to low-frequency consonants (Berlin 2006), which would show the same tendency as other bilabial consonants [b, m]. Moreover, the results for [w] in the image of cuteness are contradictory to those in the image of softness in English. Rather, it was unpredicted that the response to [j] <y> was significant, which implies that the alveolar [j] could be associated with the image of cuteness. This needs to be examined with new experiments, since it has the aforementioned problem of the ambiguous meaning of cuteness.
Fourth, apart from phonetic features, the profile of Japanese and English speakers was somewhat different: all the Japanese participants (N = 82) were female or aged under 30 years, while there were only 14 profiles that overlapped in terms of age and gender among the English speakers, which might be responsible for the different results in sound symbolic effects. The author assumed that such a difference did not affect participants’ responses, as differences in gender and age in sound symbolism are understudied (cf. Klink 2009; Kawahara et al. 2020b). However, a study in psychology showed that females were more sensitive to cute infants (Lobmaier et al. 2010); thus, there is no denying that gender differences influence responses to the notion of cuteness. Thus, how differences in gender and age arise in the judgement of softness and cuteness would be an interesting avenue for further research.
Finally, the response to the labiodental [f] was lower than a chance level. If the sound-symbolic association of cuteness resulted from the pouting gesture with the (upper and lower) lips in articulating bilabial consonants, it is convincing that the labiodental [f] does not evoke such an image, as it involves only the lower lip and not both lips. On the other hand, like the image of softness, the response to the alveolar [s] was significant for the image of cuteness. However, it is unclear whether the alveolar [s] itself evokes the images of softness and cuteness. Thus, it seems best to refrain from drawing a conclusion until future experiments are conducted. To sum up, it is safe to conclude that the consonants to express cuteness in English are unaspirated, low-frequency bilabials.
3.3 The place of articulation of the glide /w/
Based on the experimental results, this section discusses whether the glide /w/ is specified with the place-of-articulation feature [labial] in Japanese and English. For the results with Japanese speakers, it is not only the bilabial consonants [p, b, m, ɸ] but also [w] that is more likely to evoke the images of softness and cuteness, which suggests that the glide /w/ shares the feature [labial] with the bilabial consonants. Moreover, precisely speaking, the feature [labial] is associated with such images in Japanese. This accords with the claim made by Kumagai and Kawahara (2017, 2020) that the glide /w/ belongs to the [labial] category.
For the results with English speakers, the glide [w] and the bilabial consonants were more likely to evoke the image of softness. This suggests that it is specified with the feature [labial] being associated with the image of softness in English. Meanwhile, the results for the image of cuteness were less clear. A possible reason for these results is that the glide /w/ shows labiality in one case but does not in another case within a single language; in fact, as mentioned in Section 1.2, both types of evidence have been reported thus far on the non-labiality of the Japanese glide /w/. Considering this peculiar behaviour, it may be unsurprising that the current results for English showed both labiality and non-labiality; however, even if this holds true, how the place of the articulation of the glide /w/ is analysed in phonological theories would be a serious challenge. To summarize, based on the current results for the image of softness, it is possible that the glide /w/ is phonologically labial in Japanese and English.
4 Concluding remarks
The experiments in this study showed that Japanese speakers associate all the bilabial consonants [p, b, m, ɸ, w] with the images of softness and cuteness. In addition, the images of softness and cuteness of bilabial consonants could be mutually linked in the minds of Japanese speakers. In contrast, the results for English were less clear; English speakers associate the bilabial consonants [ph, p, b, m, w] with the image of softness, while they associated some of the bilabial consonants (i.e. unaspirated, low-frequency bilabials) [p, b, m] with the image of cuteness. Overall, the current study demonstrated the pluripotentiality of sound symbolism, meaning that a set of linguistic sounds conveys multiple meanings both within a single language and across languages (Winter et al. 2019; Kawahara and Kumagai to appear) and also suggested that the glide /w/ is phonologically labial in Japanese and English.
A number of issues are remaining, which could possibly be addressed in the future research. First, the notion of a word in a language is not always the same as that in another. In fact, it seems that it is difficult to find a word in foreign languages that corresponds with the Japanese word kawaii (Buckley 2016). In the case of the current study, the meaning of cuteness in English is different from the notion of kawaii in Japanese, which may have caused the difference in results. Thus, it is necessary to examine the image of cuteness/kawaii by providing a clear definition of a particular word, based on experimental reports in psychology, such as Nittono (2016). Second, the current study assumed that the image of softness resulted from the physiological reason that lips are softer than other articulation-related organs such as the alveolar ridge and the velum in the oral cavity (Kumagai et al. to appear) and that the image of cuteness resulted from the pouting gesture that appears in articulating bilabial consonants. In consideration of these aspects, it is expected that such images are likely found across languages. Other languages should be tested in the future. Third, the current study suggests the possibility that the image evoked by a particular set of sounds arises from another image by the same set of sounds. Thus, since there is a possibility that new semantic associations can be discovered, it is worth examining the pluripotentiality of bilabial consonants across natural languages.
Acknowledgements
I would like to thank Shigeto Kawahara, Kazuko Shinohara, Ryoko Uno, as well as two anonymous reviewers, for useful discussion and helpful comments. My appreciation also goes to Keiko Nakamura for her advice on the design of the experiment for English speakers. The current study is supported by a Grant-in-Aid for Early-Career Scientists from JSPS (#19K13164).
References
Akita, Kimi. 2015. “Sound symbolism.” In Handbook of pragmatics, installment 2015, ed. Jan-Ola Östman and Jef Verschueren. Amsterdam and Philadelphia: John Benjamins.10.1075/hop.19.sou1Search in Google Scholar
Akita, Kimi and Bonnie McLean. to appear. “A contrastive study of Japanese and English speakers’ sound-symbolic perception: a semantic-map approach.” In Experimental cognitive linguistics, ed. Kazuko Shinohara and Ryoko Uno. Tokyo: Hituzi Syobo [in Japanese]. https://docs.google.com/viewer?a=v&pid=sites&srcid=ZGVmYXVsdGRvbWFpbnxha2l0YW1ib3xneDoyYTJjNGMyZjM3NDE2MmJi.Search in Google Scholar
Anderson, S. R. 1976. “On the description of multiply-articulated consonants.” Journal of Phonetics 4: 17–27.10.1016/S0095-4470(19)31218-5Search in Google Scholar
Berlin, Brent. 2006. “The first congress of ethonozoological nomenclature.” Journal of Royal Anthropological Institution 12: 23–44.10.1111/j.1467-9655.2006.00271.xSearch in Google Scholar
Blasi, Damián E., Søren Wichmann, Harald Hammarström, Peter F. Stadler, and Morten H. Christianson. 2016. “Sound-meaning association biases evidenced across thousands of languages.” PNAS 113(39): 10818–23.10.1073/pnas.1605782113Search in Google Scholar
Bolts, Marilyn G., Grace M. Mangigian, and Molly B. Allen. 2016. Phonetic symbolism and memory for advertisements.” Applied Cognitive Psychology 30(6): 1088–92.10.1002/acp.3284Search in Google Scholar
Buckley, Ralf C. 2016. “Aww: the emotion of perceiving cuteness.” Frontiers in Psychology 7: 1740. 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01740.Search in Google Scholar
Bye, Patrik. 2011. “Dissimilation.” In Companion to phonology, ed. Marc van Oostendorp, Colin J. Ewen, Elizabeth V. Hume, and Keren Rice, 1408–33. Oxford: Wiley-Blackwell.10.1002/9781444335262.wbctp0060Search in Google Scholar
Carr, Philip. 2020. English phonetics and phonology: an introduction, 3rd edn. Oxford: Weley-Blackwell.Search in Google Scholar
Clements, George N. 1986. “Compensatory lengthening and consonant gemination in LuGanda.” Studies in compensatory lengthening, ed. Leo Wetzels and Engin Sezer, 37–77. Dordrecht: Foris.10.1515/9783110821666-004Search in Google Scholar
Cole, Desmond. 1967. Some features of Ganda linguistic structure. Johannesburg: Witwatersrand University Press.Search in Google Scholar
Collins, Beverley, Inger M. Mees, and Paul Carley. 2019. Practical English phonetics and phonology. London: Routledge.10.4324/9780429490392Search in Google Scholar
Cruttenden, Alan. 2014. Gimson’s pronunciation of English, 8th edn. London: Routledge.10.4324/9780203784969Search in Google Scholar
Cutler, Anne, James McQueen, and Ken Robinson. 1990. “Elizabeth and John: sound patterns of men’s and women’s names.” Journal of Linguistics 26: 471–82.10.1017/S0022226700014754Search in Google Scholar
de Lacy, Paul. 2009. “Phonological evidence.” In Phonological argumentation: Essays on evidence and motivation, ed. Steve Parker, 43–78. Equinox Publications.Search in Google Scholar
de Saussure, Ferdinand. 1916. Cours de linguistique générale. Paris: Payot.Search in Google Scholar
D’Onofrio, Annette. 2014. “Phonetic detail and dimensionality in sound-shape correspondences: refining the Bouba-Kiki paradigm.” Language and Speech 57(3): 367–93.10.1177/0023830913507694Search in Google Scholar
French, Patrice L. 1977. “Towards an explanation of phonetic symbolism.” Word 28: 305–22. 10.1080/00437956.1977.11435647.Search in Google Scholar
Gallace, Alberto, Erica Boschin, and Charles Spence. 2011. “On the taste of ‘Bouba’ and ‘Kiki’: an exploration of word-food associations in neurologically normal participants.” Cognitive Neuroscience 2: 34–46. 10. 1080/17588928.2010.516820.Search in Google Scholar
Gick, Bryan. 2003. “Articulatory correlates of ambisyllabicity in English glides and liquids.” In Papers in Laboratory Phonology VI: Constraints on Phonetic Interpretation, ed. J. Local, R. Ogden and R. Temple, 222–36. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Search in Google Scholar
Giegerich, Heinz J. 1992. English phonology: an introduction. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.10.1017/CBO9781139166126Search in Google Scholar
Hammond, Michael. 1999. The phonology of English: a prosodic optimality-theoretic approach. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Search in Google Scholar
Harris, John. 1994. English sound structure. Oxford: Blackwell.Search in Google Scholar
Hinton, Leane, Johanna Nichols, and John Ohala. 1994/2006. Sound symbolism, 2nd edn. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.10.1017/CBO9780511751806Search in Google Scholar
Hosokawa, Yuta, Naho Atsumi, Ryoko Uno, and Kazuko Shinohara. 2018. Evil or not? Sound symbolism in Pokémon and Disney character names. Poster presented at the 1st Conference on Pokémonastics. Keio University, Japan.Search in Google Scholar
Jakobson, Roman. 1941/1968. Kindersprache, Aphasie und allegemeine Lautgesetze (Child language, aphasia and phonological universals. The Hague: Mouton.10.1515/9783111353562Search in Google Scholar
Jang, Hayeun. 2018. Register-specific phonology in core grammar: Expressive strategies in Korean Aegyo. 26th Japanese/Korean Linguistics.Search in Google Scholar
Kawahara, Shigeto. 2007. Sonorancy and geminacy. University of Massachusetts Occasional Papers in Linguistics 32: Papers in Optimality III, 145–186. Amherst: GLSA.Search in Google Scholar
Kawahara, Shigeto. 2017. “A” ha “i” yorimo ookii? Onsyotyo de manabu onseigaku nyuumon [Introductory phonetics through sound symbolism]. Tokyo: Hituzi Shobo.Search in Google Scholar
Kawahara, Shigeto. 2019. What’s in a PreCure name? ICU Working Papers in Linguistics 7: Festschrift for Professor Tomoyuki Yoshida on his 60th birthday, 15–22.Search in Google Scholar
Kawahara, Shigeto. 2020. “Sound symbolism and theoretical phonology.” Language and Linguistic Compass e12372. 10.1111/lnc3.12372.Search in Google Scholar
Kawahara, Shigeto and Gakuji Kumagai. 2019a. “Inferring Pokémon types using sound symbolism: the effects of voicing and labiality.” Onsei Kenkyu [Journal of the Phonetic Society of Japan] 23(2): 111–6.Search in Google Scholar
Kawahara, Shigeto and Gakuji Kumagai. 2019b. “Expressing evolution in Pokémon names: experimental explorations.” Journal of Japanese Linguistics 35(1): 3–38.10.1515/jjl-2019-2002Search in Google Scholar
Kawahara, Shigeto and Gakuji Kumagai. to appear. “What voiced obstruents symbolically represent in Japanese: evidence from the Pokémon universe.” Journal of Japanese Linguistics. http://user.keio.ac.jp/∼kawahara/pdf/pokemonParametersFinal.pdf.10.1515/jjl-2021-2031Search in Google Scholar
Kawahara, Shigeto and Jeff Moore. to appear. “How to express evolution in English Pokémon names.” Linguistics. http://user.keio.ac.jp/∼kawahara/pdf/PokemonEnglishExperiment_v06.pdf.10.1515/ling-2021-0057Search in Google Scholar
Kawahara, Shigeto, Atsushi Noto, and Gakuji Kumagai. 2018. “Sound symbolic patterns in Pokémon names.” Phonetica 75(3): 219–244.10.1159/000484938Search in Google Scholar
Kawahara, Shigeto, Kazuko Shinohara, and Joseph Grady. 2015. “Iconic inferences about personality: from sounds and shapes.” In Iconicity: East Meets West, ed. Masako Hiraga, W. Herlofsky, K. Shinohara, and Kimi Akita, 57–69. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.10.1075/ill.14.03kawSearch in Google Scholar
Kawahara, Shigeto, Gakuji Kumagai, and Mahayana C. Godoy. 2020a. English speakers can infer Pokémon types using sound symbolism. Ms. Keio University, Meikai University and Federal University of Rio Grande do Norte.10.3389/fpsyg.2021.648948Search in Google Scholar
Kawahara, Shigeto, Mahayana C. Godoy, and Gakuji Kumagai. 2020b. “Do sibilants fly? Evidence from a sound symbolic pattern in Pokémon names.” Open Linguistics 6(1): 386–400.10.1515/opli-2020-0027Search in Google Scholar
Kingston, John and Randy L. Diehl. 1994. “Phonetic knowledge.” Language 70: 419–54.10.1353/lan.1994.0023Search in Google Scholar
Klink, Richard R. 2000. “Creating brand names with meaning: the use of sound symbolism.” Marketing Letters 11: 5–20.10.1023/A:1008184423824Search in Google Scholar
Klink, Richard R. 2009. “Gender differences in new brand name response.” Marketing Letters 20: 313–26.10.1007/s11002-008-9066-xSearch in Google Scholar
Köhler, Woldgang. 1929. Gestalt psychology. New York, NY: Liveright.Search in Google Scholar
Kubozono, Haruo. 2015. “Introduction to Japanese phonetics and phonology.” In The handbook of Japanese language and linguistics: phonetics and phonology, ed. Haruo Kubozono, 1–40. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.10.1515/9781614511984.1Search in Google Scholar
Kumagai, Gakuji. 2017. Testing the OCP-labial effect on Japanese rendaku. Ms. Meikai University. Downloadable at: https://ling.auf.net/lingbuzz/003290.Search in Google Scholar
Kumagai, Gakuji. 2019. “A sound-symbolic alternation to express cuteness and the orthographic Lyman’s Law in Japanese.” Journal of Japanese Linguistics 35(1): 39–74.10.1515/jjl-2019-2004Search in Google Scholar
Kumagai, Gakuji and Shigeto Kawahara. 2017. “How abstract is sound symbolism? Labiality and diaper names in Japanese.” Proceedings of the 31st Annual Meeting of the Phonetic Society of Japan, 49–54 [in Japanese].Search in Google Scholar
Kumagai, Gakuji and Shigeto Kawahara. 2020. “Feature-based sound symbolism: Labiality and diaper names in Japanese.” Gengo Kenkyu [Journal of the Linguistic Society of Japan] 157: 149–61. [in Japanese].Search in Google Scholar
Kumagai, Gakuji, Kazuaki Yoshitake, Hiromu Tanji, and Takuya Matsuhashi. 2020. “Sound-symbolic effects of voiced obstruents and mora counts on the monster names of Digital Monster and Monster Hunter and on the spell names of Final Fantasy.” Onsei Kenkyu [Journal of the Phonetic Society of Japan] 24(2): 63–70.Search in Google Scholar
Kumagai, Gakuji, Ryoko Uno, and Kazuko Shinohara. to appear. “The sound-symbolic effects of consonants on food texture: an experimental study of snack names in Japanese.” In The language of Japanese food: Cognitive perspectives and beyond, ed. Kiyoko Toratani. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.10.1075/celcr.25.03kumSearch in Google Scholar
Labrune, Lawrence. 2012. The phonology of Japanese. Oxford: Oxford University Press.10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199545834.001.0001Search in Google Scholar
Leben, William R. 1973. Suprasegmental phonology. Cambridge, MA: Massachusetts Institute Technology dissertation.Search in Google Scholar
Levi, Susannah V. 2008. “Phonemic vs. derived glides.” Lingua 118: 1956–78.10.1016/j.lingua.2007.10.003Search in Google Scholar
Lobmaier, J. S., R. Sprengelmeyer, B. Wiffen, and D. I. Perrett. 2010. “Female and male perception of cuteness, age, and emotion in infant faces.” Evolution and Human Behavior 31(1): 16–21.10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2009.05.004Search in Google Scholar
Lockwood, Gwilym and Mark Dingemanse. 2015. “Iconicity in the lab: a review of behavioral, developmental, and neuroimaging research into sound-symbolism.” Frontiers in Psychology 6: 1246.Search in Google Scholar
Lowrey, Tian M. and L. J. Shrum. 2007. “Phonetic symbolism and brand name preference.” Journal of Consumer Research 34: 406–14.10.1086/518530Search in Google Scholar
MacNeilage, Peter F., Barbara L. Davis, and Christine L. Matyear. 1994. “Babbling and first words: Phonetic similarities and differences.” Speech Communication 22: 269–77.10.1016/S0167-6393(97)00022-8Search in Google Scholar
McCarthy, John J. 1986. “OCP effects: gemination and antigemination.” Linguistic Inquiry 17(2): 207–63.Search in Google Scholar
McCarthy, John J. 2002. A thematic guide to Optimality Theory. Cambridge, Cambridge University Press.10.1017/CBO9780511613333Search in Google Scholar
McCarthy, John J. 2008. Doing Optimality Theory: Applying theory to data. New York: Blackwell.10.1002/9781444301182Search in Google Scholar
Nevins, Andrew and Ioana Chitoran. 2008. “Phonological representations and the variable patterning of glides.” Lingua 118: 1979–97.10.1016/j.lingua.2007.10.006Search in Google Scholar
Newman, Stanley S. 1933. “Further experiments on phonetic symbolism.” American Journal of Psychology 45: 53–75.10.2307/1414186Search in Google Scholar
Nittono, Hiroshi. 2016. “The two-layer model of “kawaii”: a behavioural science framework for understanding kawaii and cuteness.” East Asian Journal of Popular Culture 2: 79–95.10.1386/eapc.2.1.79_1Search in Google Scholar
Nomura, Michio. 2010. Naze ahiruguti ni hikarerunoka [Why is the Japanese duck mouth appealing]? Tokyo: Media Factory.Search in Google Scholar
Nuckolls, Janis B. 1999. “The case for sound symbolism.” Annual Review of Anthropology 28: 225–52.10.1146/annurev.anthro.28.1.225Search in Google Scholar
Ohala, John J. 1984. “An ethological perspective on common cross-language utilization of F0 of voice.” Phonetica 41: 1–16.10.1159/000261706Search in Google Scholar
Ohala, John J. 1994. “The frequency code underlies the sound symbolic use of voice pitch.” In Sound symbolism, ed. Leane Hinton, Jojanna Nichols and John J. Ohala, 325–47.10.1017/CBO9780511751806.022Search in Google Scholar
Ohala, John and James Lorentz. 1977. “The story of [w]: an exercise in the phonetic explanation for sound patterns.” Proceedings of the Berkeley Linguistic Society 3: 577–99.10.3765/bls.v3i0.2264Search in Google Scholar
Paradis, Carole. 1987. “Glide alternations in Pulaar (Fula) and the Theory of Charm and Government.” In Current approaches to African linguistics, ed. David Odden, 327–39. Dordrecht: Foris.10.1515/9783110882681-025Search in Google Scholar
Pintér, Gábor. 2015. “The emergence of new consonant contrasts.” In The handbook of Japanese language and linguistics: phonetics and phonology, ed. Haruo Kubozono, 121–66. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.10.1515/9781614511984.121Search in Google Scholar
Pitcher, Benjamin J., Alex Mesoudi, and Alan G. McElligott. 2013. “Sex-biased sound symbolism in English-language first names.” PLoS One 8(6): e64825. 10.1371/jounal.pone.0064825.Search in Google Scholar
Peterson, Robert A. and Ivan Ross. 1972. “How to name new brands.” Journal of Advertising Research 12: 29–34.Search in Google Scholar
Pogacar, Ruth, Emily Plant, Laura Felton Rosulek, and Michal Kouril. 2015. “Sounds good: phonetic sound patterns in top brand names.” Marketing Letters 26: 549–63.10.1007/s11002-014-9288-zSearch in Google Scholar
Prince, Alan and Paul Smolensky. 1993/2004. Optimality Theory: Constraint interaction in generative grammar. Malden, MA & Oxford, UK: Blackwell.10.1002/9780470759400Search in Google Scholar
Qiu, Lin, Jiahui Lu, Shanshan Yang, Weina Qu, and Tingshao Zhu. 2015. “What does your selfie say about you?” Computers in Human Behavior 52: 443–9.10.1016/j.chb.2015.06.032Search in Google Scholar
Reetz, Henning and Allard Jongman. 2009. Phonetics: transcription, production, acoustics, and perception. Oxford: Wiley-Blackwell.Search in Google Scholar
Roach, Peter. 2009. English phonetics and phonology: a practical course. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Search in Google Scholar
Sapir, Edward. 1929. “A study in phonetic symbolism.” Journal of Experimental Psychology 12: 225–39.10.1037/h0070931Search in Google Scholar
Shibatani, Masayoshi. 1990. The language of Japan. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Search in Google Scholar
Shih, Stephanie S., Jordan Ackerman, Noah Hermalin, Sharon Inkelas, and Darya Kavitskaya. 2018. “Pokémonikers: a study of sound symbolism and Pokémon names.” Proceedings of Linguistic Society of America 3(42): 1–6.10.3765/plsa.v3i1.4335Search in Google Scholar
Shih, Stephanie S., Jordan Ackerman, Noah Hermalin, Sharon Inkelas, Hayeun Jang, Jessica Johnson, Darya Kavitskaya, Shigeto Kawahara, Miran Oh, Rebecca L. Starre, and Alan Yu. 2019. Cross-linguistic and language-specific sound symbolism: Pokémonastics. Ms. Downloadable at https://ling.auf.net/lingbuzz/004725.Search in Google Scholar
Shinohara, Kazuko and Shigeto Kawahara. 2013. “The sound symbolic nature of Japanese maid names.” Proceedings of the 13th Annual Meeting of the Japanese Cognitive Linguistics Association 13: 183–93.Search in Google Scholar
Shinohara, Kazuko and Shigeto Kawahara. 2016. “A cross-linguistic study of sound symbolism: the images of size.” Proceedings of the 36th Annual Meeting of the Berkeley Linguistics Society, 396–410. Berkeley: Berkeley Linguistics Society.10.3765/bls.v36i1.3926Search in Google Scholar
Sidhu, David M. and Penny M. Pexman. 2018. “Five mechanisms of sound symbolic association.” Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 25(5): 1619–43.10.3758/s13423-017-1361-1Search in Google Scholar
Sidhu, David M. and Penny M. Pexman. 2019. “The sound symbolism of names.” Current Directions in Psychological Science 28(4): 398–402.10.1177/0963721419850134Search in Google Scholar
Sidhu, David M., Kristen Deschamps, Joshua S. Bourdage, and Penny M. Pexman. 2019. “Does the name say it all? Investigating phoneme-personality sound symbolism in first names.” Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 148(9): 1595–614.10.1037/xge0000662Search in Google Scholar
Suzuki, Keiichiro. 1998. A typological investigation of dissimilation. Arizona: University of Arizona dissertation.Search in Google Scholar
Tessier, Anne-Michelle. 2016. Phonological acquisition: Child language and constraint-based grammar. Basingstoke: Palgrave Macmillan.10.1007/978-1-137-54306-6Search in Google Scholar
Tsujimura, Natsuko. 2014. An introduction to Japanese linguistics. 3rd edn. Oxford: Blackwell.Search in Google Scholar
Ultan, Russel. 1978. “Size-sound symbolism.” In Universals of Human Language II: Phonology, ed. Joseph Greenberg, 525–68. Stanford: Stanford University Press.Search in Google Scholar
Uno, Ryoko, Kazuko Shinohara, Yuta Hosokawa, Naho Atsumi, Gakuji Kumagai, and Shigeto Kawahara. 2020. “What’s in a villain’s name? Sound symbolic values of voiced obstruents and bilabial consonants.” Review of Cognitive Linguistics 18(2): 428–57.10.1075/rcl.00066.unoSearch in Google Scholar
Urban, M. 2011. “Conventional sound symbolism in terms for organs of speech: a cross-linguistic study.” Folia Linguist 45: 199–214. 10.1515/flin.2011.007.Search in Google Scholar
Vance, Timothy J. 1987. An introduction to Japanese phonology. New York: SUNY Press.Search in Google Scholar
Vance, Timothy J. 2015. “Rendaku.” In The handbook of Japanese language and linguistics: phonetics and phonology, ed. Haruo Kubozono, 397–441. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.10.1515/9781614511984.397Search in Google Scholar
Vance, Timothy J. and Mark Irwin (ed.). 2016. Sequential voicing in Japanese compounds: Papers from the NINJAL Rendaku Project. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.10.1075/slcs.176Search in Google Scholar
Wichmann, Søren, Eric W. Holman, and Cecil H. Brown. 2010. “Sound symbolism in basic vocabulary.” Entropy 12: 844–58.10.3390/e12040844Search in Google Scholar
Winter, Bodo, Paula Perez-Sobrino, and Lucien Brown. 2019. “The sound of soft alcohol: crossmodal associations between interjections and liquor.” PLoS One 14(8): e0220449. 10.1371/journal.pone.0220449.Search in Google Scholar
Wright, Saundra K., Jennifer Hay, and Tessa Bent. 2005. “Ladies first? Phonology, frequency, and the naming conspiracy.” Linguistics 43(3): 531–61.10.1515/ling.2005.43.3.531Search in Google Scholar
Yamaguchi, Toshiko. 2007. Japanese linguistics: an introduction. London: Continuum.Search in Google Scholar
Yavas, Mahmet. 2016. Applied English phonology. 3rd edn. Oxford: Blackwell.Search in Google Scholar
Yip, Moira. 1988. “The Obligatory Contour Principle and phonological rules: a loss of identity.” Linguistic Inquiry 19(1): 65–100.Search in Google Scholar
Yorkston, Eric and Geeta Menon. 2004. “A sound idea: phonetic effects of brand names on consumer judgement.” Journal of Consumer Research 31: 43–51.10.1086/383422Search in Google Scholar
© 2020 Gakuji Kumagai, published by De Gruyter
This work is licensed under the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License.