Skip to content
BY 4.0 license Open Access Published by De Gruyter Open Access June 27, 2022

Reconstruction of Ryukyuan tone classes of Middle Japanese Class 2.4 and 2.5 nouns

  • Yosuke Igarashi EMAIL logo
From the journal Open Linguistics

Abstract

It is generally accepted that of the five tone classes reconstructed for disyllabic nouns in Middle Japanese (MJ), Classes 2.4 and 2.5 exhibit a split correspondence with proto-Ryukyuan (pR) tone Classes B and C. This split correspondence is of tremendous importance for the reconstruction of the proto-Japonic (pJ) tone system, because, in the absence of a conditioning factor accounting for the split, it has led to the reconstruction of additional tone classes at the pJ level. However, of the approximately 100 nouns belonging to these classes, the tone class of only half of them has been reconstructed for pR. Before embarking on the reconstruction of the pJ tone system, we must therefore first reconstruct the pR tone class of the nouns belonging to MJ Classes 2.4 and 2.5. This study provides a reconstruction of the tone classes in pR for 75 cognates using the latest comparative data of Northern and Southern Ryukyuan dialects. The results confirmed the existence of a split correspondence, where Class 2.4 and 2.5 nouns are found to be roughly split in half between pR Classes B and C, demonstrating that it is not a merely sporadic irregularity.

1 Introduction

According to the currently widely accepted phylogenetic classification (Pellard 2015), the Japonic language family consists of three branches, namely, Ryukyuan, Japanese, and Hachijo, although the phylogenetic position of Hachijo is debatable. The Ryukyuan languages are spoken in the Ryukyu Islands and consist of five mutually unintelligible languages. They are classified into two subbranches, Northern and Southern Ryukyuan. The former comprises the Amami and Okinawa languages, whereas the latter the Miyako, Yaeyama, and Yonaguni languages (Pellard 2015). The common ancestor of Ryukyuan, Japanese, and Hachijo is called proto-Japonic (pJ), whereas that of the Ryukyuan languages is referred to as proto-Ryukyuan (pR). The terms Old Japanese (OJ) and Middle Japanese (MJ) refer to the dialect spoken around the ancient capital (Nara and Kyoto) in the eighth and eleventh centuries, respectively. The eighth-century written records of Japanese include dialects from around the ancient capital (West Old Japanese) and Eastern Japan (East Old Japanese), but this study does not discuss East Old Japanese, and the term OJ refers only to West Old Japanese.

Although the phonograms in the OJ text Nihon Shoki have been argued to partly reflect the tonal OJ contrasts (e.g., Takayama 2003), MJ is the oldest stage of Japanese for which we have both extensive and accurate materials on tonal contrasts. Based on these extensive materials, along with a comparison of modern Japanese dialects, the standard theory (Kindaichi 1974) reconstructs five tone classes for disyllabic nouns in MJ, commonly labeled Classes 2.1, 2.2, 2.3, 2.4, and 2.5. Conversely, the comparison of Ryukyuan dialects has led to the reconstruction of three tone classes in pR, namely, Classes A, B, and C (Hattori 1958, Matsumori 2012).

The correspondences between the tone classes of MJ and pR are not, however, all straightforward. Although some studies (Kindaichi 1960, Hirayama et al. 1966, 1967, Uemura, 1977) state that MJ Classes 2.1 and 2.2 correspond to pR Class A, MJ Class 2.3 to pR Class B, and MJ Class 2.4–2.5 to pR Class C, to date, not a single Ryukyuan dialect has been found where this holds true. As first discovered by Hattori (1958), and repeatedly confirmed by subsequent studies (Hattori 1979a,b, Matsumori 1998, 2000a,b, 2009, 2010, 2012, Shimabukuro 2007, de Boer 2010), MJ Classes 2.4 and 2.5 (Class 2.4–2.5, henceforth) exhibit a split correspondence with pR Classes B and C. In other words, although MJ Classes 2.1 and 2.2 regularly correspond to pR Class A, and MJ Class 2.3 to pR Class B,[1] MJ Class 2.4–2.5 nouns are split roughly in half between pR Classes B and C (Table 1).[2] The split correspondence in MJ Class 2.4–2.5 has led researchers to define additional subclasses for disyllabic nouns (Hattori 1979a,b); namely, Classes 2.4a and 2.5a (Class 2.4a/2.5a, henceforth), whose pR reflex is Class C, and Classes 2.4b and 2.5b (Class 2.4b/2.5b, henceforth), whose pR reflex is Class B.

Table 1

Tonal correspondences between Japanese dialects (Kyoto and Oita [Hirayama et al. (1992–1993)]) and Ryukyuan dialects (Yoron [Kiku and Takahashi 2005] and Nakijin [Nakasone 1983]). Nouns in the Oita dialect are accompanied by the case particle -ga because in this dialect, part of the tonal distinction is neutralized unless another morpheme follows the noun

MJ classes pR classes Subclasses Kyoto Oita Yoron Nakijin
2.1 A “nose” háná haná-gá pana pʰanáá
“water” mízú midú-gá miʑi midʑíí
2.2 A “sound” óto otó-gá utu ɸut'úú
“snow” júki jukí-gá juki jutʑ’íí
2.3 B “mountain” jáma jamá-ga jamá jamaá
“year” tóɕi toɕí-ga tuɕí tʰuɕií
2.4 C 2.4a “middle” naká náka-ga náá náhaa
“breath” ikí íki-ga íkí ʔítʑ’i
B 2.4b “shoulder” katá káta-ga hatá hat’aá
“barley” mugí múgi-ga mugí mudʑií
2.5 C 2.5a “pot” nabê nábe-ga nábí nábi
“monkey” sarû sáru-ga sárú sáru
B 2.5b “rain” amê áme-ga amí amií
“sweat” asê áse-ga aɕí aɕií

Some researchers have advanced the hypothesis that the split correspondence is the result of a conditioned sound change that occurred in pR, whereby Class 2.4a/2.5a nouns merged into Class 2.3 (Kindaichi 1960, Hirayama et al. 1966, Tokugawa 1990). This conditioned merger hypothesis is, however, not widely accepted today, because it suffers from too many exceptions (Uwano 1996a, de Boer 2010). In the absence of a clear conditioning factor, most researchers interpret the distinction between Class 2.4a/2.5a and Class 2.4b/2.5b in pR as an archaic feature inherited from pJ (e.g., Hattori 1979a,b, Vovin 1993, 2008). Thus, there has been an intense debate as to what tonal contrasts should be reconstructed for pR and pJ (Shimabukuro 2008, Vovin 2008, Pellard 2009b, Uwano 2017b).

However, the exact membership of the subclasses has not been fully elucidated. Indeed, although approximately 100 nouns have already been identified as belonging to Class 2.4–2.5 (Kindaichi 1974), for only half of them has the tone class of pR been reconstructed so far (Hattori 1979a,b, Matsumori 2000b, 2009, 2012, Shimabukuro 2007, de Boer 2010). A further problem with previous research (Hattori 1979a,b, Matsumori 2009, 2012) is that the reconstructions were primarily based on data from Northern Ryukyuan, whereas Southern Ryukyuan played little role. Before we can begin to have a fruitful conversation about the pJ tone system, the exact makeup of the subclasses must first be firmly established. The enlargement of the inventory of Class 2.4–2.5 nouns in pR would either strengthen the widely accepted hypothesis that regards the subclasses as archaisms inherited from pJ or revive the unorthodox hypothesis that views them as an innovation in pR.

This study thus aims to determine the membership of Classes 2.4a/2.5a and 2.5a/2.5b based on the latest data available for Northern and Southern Ryukyuan. Section 2 lays out the background. Sections 3 and 4 describe the methods and results of our survey, respectively. Summarizing the results, Section 4 reveals that the split correspondence is genuine, and it reexamines the hypothesis that views it as a result of a conditioned merger. Section 5 concludes this study.

2 Background

The split correspondence in Class 2.4–2.5 was first discovered by Hattori (1958). To confirm his findings, Hattori (1979a,b) examined 10 dialects of Northern Ryukyuan and reconstructed the pR tone classes of 37 Class 2.4–2.5 nouns[3] (Table 2). Based on data from several dialects of Northern Ryukyuan, Matsumori (1998, 2000a) revealed that the split correspondence was consistently observed in several dialects, confirming that it was genuine, and not merely a sporadic irregularity. In addition, Matsumori (2000b, 2009, 2012) proposed a vocabulary list called keiretsubestu goi, in which pR tone classes of more than 400 nouns are reconstructed. Of these nouns, 35 (Matsumori 2009) and 27 (Matsumori 2012) nouns belong to Class 2.4–2.5 (Table 2).[4]

Table 2

Dialects and the number of Class 2.4–2.5 nouns analyzed in previous studies

Branches Languages Dialects (source) No. Class 2.4–2.5 nouns
Hattori (1979a,b) Northern Amami Onotsu, Aden, Naze, Shodon, Asama (Uwano 1977) 37
Okinawa Nakijin, Kushi, Onna, Shuri (OKJ 1963)
Matsumori (2009) Northern Okinawa Kin 35
Matsumori (2012) Northern Amami Akaren, China 27
Okinawa Kin
Shimabukuro (2007) Northern Amami Naze (Hirayama et al. 1966, 1967), Kamishiro (ASB 1959), Kametsu (Hattori 1979a,b) 13
Okinawa Nakijin (Nakasone 1983), Shuri (OKJ 1963), Aguni (Hirayama et al. 1966)
Southern Miyako Ikema (Hirayama et al. 1967), Oura (Hirayama et al. 1967), Uechi (Hirayama et al. 1967)
Yaeyama Ishigaki (Hirayama et al. 1967), Sonai (Hirayama et al. 1967), Kuroshima (Hirayama et al. 1967)
Yonaguni Yonaguni (Hirayama et al. 1967)
de Boer (2010) Northern Amami Aden (Hattori 1979a,b), Ashikebu (Uwano 1996a), Naze (Hattori (1979a,b), Asama (Uwano 1977 as cited in Hattori 1979a,b), Tokuwase (Matsumori 1998), Wadomari (Kuno 1991) 42
Okinawa Nakijin (Hattori 1979a,b), Onna (Hattori 1979a,b), Shuri (Hattori 1979a,b)
Southern Miyako Ikema-Sarahama (Matsumori 1998)
Yaeyama Hateruma (Hirayama 1988)
Yonaguni Yonaguni (Hirayama 1988, Martin 1987)

Unlike the studies mentioned above, which are mainly based on Northern Ryukyuan, Shimabukuro (2007) included data from both Northern and Southern Ryukyuan in his comparison, but he only analyzed 13 MJ Class 2.4–2.5 nouns (Table 2), which were a subset of Hattori’s (1979a,b) 37 nouns. de Boer (2010) also compared both Northern and Southern Ryukyuan, but her vocabulary considerably overlaps with Hattori’s (1979a,b); she removed two words from Hattori (1979a,b) and newly added seven. Thus, the total number of MJ Class 2.4–2.5 nouns in de Boer (2010) was 42 (Table 2).[5]

The vocabulary investigated by previous studies is limited. Although Kindaichi (1974) lists 108 nouns as belonging to Class 2.4–2.5, the pR tone classes of only 55 of them (measured by type frequency) have hitherto been analyzed. This does not mean that the rest of Class 2.4–2.5 nouns do not exist in pR. On the contrary, as Section 4.4 reveals, at least 93 Class 2.4–2.5 nouns are considered to have existed in pR. This study thus attempts to establish the pR tone classes of those nouns that have not been reconstructed in previous studies so as to enlarge the inventory of Class 2.4–2.5 nouns in pR.

It is also clear from the discussion above that the previous literature has been based primarily on Northern Ryukyuan. In some respect, it was inevitable that Southern Ryukyuan was neglected. First, except for the Yonaguni dialect (Hirayama and Nakamoto 1964), dialects of Southern Ryukyuan distinguishing the three pR tone classes were not discovered until Matsumori’s (2010) study of the Tarama dialect. Second, although many Southern Ryukyuan dialects retaining the three-way tonal contrast of pR were discovered one after another in the 2010s (Igarashi et al. 2011, Igarashi et al., 2012, Matsumori 2012, 2013, 2015), at the same time, the prosodic systems of these dialects turned out to be extremely complex. Indeed, because of extensive tonal neutralization processes at work in these dialects, the observation of words pronounced in isolation, or even embedded in short carrier sentences, proved completely insufficient for the correct identification of the tone classes to which each word belongs (Igarashi 2016a, Matsumori 2016). As a result, a tremendous amount of elicitation is necessary to determine accurately the tone class of each word in these dialects. Fortunately, extensive data on tone class membership have recently been made available for at least two Southern Ryukyuan dialects that preserve the three tone classes of pR; namely, the Ikema dialect of Miyako (Igarashi 2016b, Igarashi et al. 2018) and especially the Tarama dialect of Miyako (Tokuyama and Celik 2020). Although extensive data on tone classes membership in Yaeyama have also become available recently (e.g., Ishigaki [Miyagi 2003], Taketomi [Maeara et al. 2011], and Hatoma [Kajiku 2020]), pR Classes B and C have merged in most Yaeyama dialects, making it impossible to use data for the reconstruction of pR tone classes. This study therefore exploits data from Ikema, Tarama, and Yonaguni as primary data for Southern Ryukyuan to establish pR tone classes from the Northern and Southern Ryukyuan perspectives.

3 Methods

3.1 Data

We exploit two types of resources for reconstructing pR tone classes, primary and secondary data. The primary data are used for the reconstruction of pR tone classes. They consist of the information regarding the tone class membership of eleven dialects, which all preserve the distinction between pR Classes B and C (Table 3). The Ashikebu and Nakasato dialects merged Classes A and B. The Ie dialect merged Classes A and C in bimoraic nouns. The Ikema dialect preserves the three-way contrast in pR, but Class A has almost completely merged with Class B for bimoraic nouns (Igarashi 2016b). The other dialects far more faithfully preserve the three-way contrast in pR. The secondary data are used to examine whether a given cognate is widely observed in Ryukyuan dialects and to confirm the correspondences in vowels and consonants between dialects. They consist of seven dialects that merged pR Classes B and C or that do not have tonal contrasts at all (Table 4).

Table 3

Primary data

Branches Languages Dialects Sources
Northern Amami Ashikebu Uwano (1996a)
Nakasato Uwano (2014)
Asama Uwano (2017)
Wadomari Uwano (2007)
Yoron (Higashiku variety) Kiku and Takahashi (2005)
Okinawa Ie Oshio (2009)
Nakijin (Yonamine variety) Nakasone (1983)
Shuri OKJ (1963)
Southern Miyako Tarama Tokuyama and Celik (2020), the author’s field notes
Ikema (Nishihara variety) The author’s field notes
Yonaguni Yonaguni Uwano (2010, 2013)
Table 4

Secondary data

Branches Languages Dialects Sources
Northern Amami Yamatohama Osada et al. (1980)
Southern Miyako Irabu (Nakachi variety) Tomihama (2013)
Yaeyama Ishigaki (Shika variety) Miyagi (2003)
Taketomi Maeara et al. (2011)
Hatoma Kajiku (2020)
Hateruma Hirayama et al. (1967)
Aragusuku Miyanaga (1930)

MJ classes are based on Kindaichi (1974), which lists a total of 108 Class 2.4–2.5 nouns. The entries of his vocabulary are written in italics in the MJ forms (Akinaga et al. 1997).

Word forms are provided in broad phonetic transcription. The diacritic “′” indicates a glottalized consonant. Long vowels are represented by a series of identical vowels. “ï,” “ɨ,” and “ɿ” are uniformly written as “ɨ.” The pairs “ʃ” and “ɕ” and “ʒ” and “ʑ” are uniformly represented as “ɕ” and “ʑ,” respectively. Moreover, the diacritics “” and “” designate a pitch rise and fall, respectively. For example, “μμ” (“μ” stands for mora) represents an LH pitch pattern (“L” and “H” stand for low and high pitch, respectively). “μμ” is distinguished from “μμ”; when produced in isolation, the two are (nearly) indistinguishable, but when immediately followed by another morpheme such as a case particle, the former is realized as LH-L, whereas the latter is realized as LH-H. In Tarama and Ikema, tonal neutralization occurs with nouns produced in isolation. Tone opposition can be observed, for example, by adding one or more bimoraic particles (one for Tarama and two for Ikema) after the noun. The -kara ‘from’ and -mai ‘also’ in the tables are bimoraic particles.

A superscript at the end of each word form indicates the correspondence with the pR tone class, where “A,” “B,” and “C” are the pitch patterns corresponding to the pR Class A, Class B, and Class C, respectively. Because of the conditioned merger that occurred in each dialect, one pitch pattern may correspond to more than one class. In that case, more than one letter is used to indicate the correspondence. For example, “AC” indicates that the pitch pattern corresponds to both pR Classes A and C, and “BC” indicates that the pitch pattern corresponds to both pR Classes B and C.

Because we use multiple sources for Tarama, there can be mismatches in the pitch pattern between the sources. In case of discrepancy, the pattern in the author’s field note is adopted first, with that in Tokuyama and Celik (2020) in parentheses; for example, “(∼A).”

3.2 Reconstruction

We postulate the phoneme inventory in Figure 1 for pR. We assume that an intervocalic *w occurred only before *a, an intervocalic *j was allowed only before a back vowel, and *d had already merged with *z before a high vowel. No obligatory glide insertion is assumed between vowels. Thus, the pR noun for “voice,” for example, is reconstructed as *koe instead of *kowe or *koje. We leave aside whether *t, *d, *s, and *z were palatalized before a high vowel. We use a capital *U in cases where we could not decide whether *o or *u was to be reconstructed. Examining the segmental correspondence between dialects is important for determining whether nouns that are similar in both meaning and form are cognate with each other. The determination of cognacy constitutes an indispensable part of the reconstruction of the pR tone class of the nouns. Furthermore, the correct reconstruction of word-final vowels in pR is crucial for testing the conditioned merger hypothesis. We discuss the issues concerning the segmental reconstruction and cognacy between nouns only if they require argumentation.

When the tonal correspondence between the dialects is irregular, we adopt the principle of parsimony, according to which the reconstruction should require the fewest sound changes. The parsimony is assessed on the basis of the phylogenetic tree proposed by Pellard (2015) (Figure 2). For example, pR *poka ‘outside’ exhibits irregular tonal correspondence (Ashikebu ɸuka AB, Asama ɸukaa B, Wadomari ɸaa B, Yoron huka C, Ie ɸuk’aa B, Nakijin ɸuk’aa B, Shuri ɸuka B, Tarama puka C, Ikema huka C, Yonaguni huga B). If we posit pR Class B, then the sporadic sound change B > C is reconstructed to occur two times, each for Yoron and proto-Miyako. By contrast, if we posit pR Class C, then the sporadic sound change C > B is reconstructed to occur five times, each for Ashikebu, Asama, Wadomari, proto-Okinawa, and Yonaguni. Thus, the principle of parsimony judges Class B as more plausible than Class C.

Figure 1 
            Proposed phoneme inventory in pR.
Figure 1

Proposed phoneme inventory in pR.

Figure 2 
            Phylogenetic tree of the Japonic language family proposed by Pellard (2015). The phylogenetic position of Hachijo is controversial, which is indicated by a dotted line.
Figure 2

Phylogenetic tree of the Japonic language family proposed by Pellard (2015). The phylogenetic position of Hachijo is controversial, which is indicated by a dotted line.

4 Results

4.1 Confirmation of tone classes reconstructed by previous studies

The survey confirmed that the tone classes reconstructed by previous studies were valid for 52 of the 55 words (Table 5). In the table, nouns that show an irregular tonal correspondence are shaded. In addition, segments that obviously show an irregular correspondence are underlined. The exceptional correspondence exhibited by quite a few dialects in nouns where cognates are found in most dialects can be regarded as the result of sporadic changes in individual dialects and does not affect the reconstruction of tone classes based on the principle of parsimony. Sporadic changes of this sort are not discussed in this section. Some nouns require further commentary, which is provided below.

Table 5

Comparison and reconstruction of Class 2.4–2.5 nouns. Ha79, Hattori (1979a,b); Sh07, Shimabukuro (2007), Ma09, Matsumori (2009), Bo10, de Boer (2010), Ma12, Matsumori (2012)

13. *sora C ‘treetop’: Matsumori (2009, 2012) also reconstructs Class C for *sora ‘treetop,’ although she may not agree with the view that pR *sora C ‘treetop’ is related to MJ sora 2.4 ‘sky.’ In the tenth century, Japanese sora referring to not only ‘sky’ but also ‘the upper part of an object (such as roof, ceiling, and treetop)’ is attested (NKDJ 2001, 507). This means that at the stage of MJ at the latest, Japanese sora also had a meaning equivalent to that of pR. This study does not examine the question of whether the pJ cognate had the meaning of ‘the upper part of an object.’ What is clear from the above discussion is that the semantic change between ‘sky’ and ‘the upper part of an object’ is a natural process, and that pR *sora ‘treetop’ is reasonably regarded as the cognate of MJ sora 2.4 ‘sky.’ Note that pR *sora does not have a meaning ‘sky,’ which is indicated instead by a Chinese loan pR *ten A.

21. *moko C ‘bridegroom’: Nakasato, Asama, and Yoron have mukk’a C ‘bridegroom,’ mukk’wa ‘id.,’ and muukwaa C ‘id.,’ respectively. They must be polymorphemic, consisting of pR *moko C ‘bridegroom’ and pR *kua A ‘child,’ or they arose through reanalysis of the second syllable of pR *moko C ‘bridegroom’ as pR *kua A ‘child.’ The regular tonal correspondence allow reconstruction of pR *moko C ‘bridegroom.’

25. *tuju C ‘dew’: Whether the pR form of ‘dew’ is *tuju or *tujo becomes clear when comparing ‘dew’ with pR *tujo- ‘strong’ in Ishigaki and Hatoma, Ishigaki tɕuu BC ‘dew’ and tsuu- ‘strong,’ and Hatoma ɕuu BC ‘dew’ and suu- ‘strong.’ They are distinguished by the palatalization of the consonant. For this, pR *tuju ‘dew’ is reconstructed. Tarama tsɨv C ‘dew’ may not seem to be a reflex of pR *tuju ‘id.’ However, the sound change pR *ju > v is confirmed by pR *suju- ‘sour,’ reflected in Tarama sɨv- ‘id.’[6] In Irabu, pR *tuju ‘dew’ and *suju- ‘sour’ are also reflected as tsɨv and sɨv-. Thus, the change pR *ju > v/*{s, t}u_ can be reconstructed in proto-Miyako.[7]

26. *woke C ‘bucket’: The correspondence of the word-initial consonant is irregular in Nakijin, Tarama, and Yonaguni, suggesting that sporadic sound changes occurred in these dialects. These changes, though irregular, are phonetically motivated at least for Nakijin and Tarama; in Nakijin, w became ɸ by assimilating its voicing to that of the consonant in the following syllable, and in Tarama, *b (<*w through a regular sound change) became ɡ by assimilating its place of articulation to the consonant in the following syllable. The tonal correspondences are regular throughout all dialects. These facts indicate that these nouns are inherited from pR, allowing us to reconstruct pR *woke C ‘bucket.’

32. *kama B ‘sickle’: Although Matsumori (2012) reconstructs Class B, only Amami exhibits pitch patterns corresponding to pR Class B according to our survey. The pitch patterns in Okinawa, in contrast, correspond to Class A. The cognates are perhaps absent in Southern Ryukyuan, where reflexes of pR *irana C ‘sickle’ are attested instead (Tarama ɨzara C, Ikema zzara C, Ishigaki ʔiɾana BC, Yonaguni irara C). They are also attested in Okinawa (Nakijin ʔinaana C, Shuri ʔirana C). Regardless of whether the pR Class of *kama is reconstructed as A or B, the number of reconstructed changes remains the same: the change A > B in proto-Amami or the change B > A in proto-Okinawa. Because Class B or C is a regular correspondence with MJ Class 2.4, it is more plausible to reconstruct pR Class B for *kama ‘sickle’ and a subsequent sporadic change B > A in proto-Okinawa.

36. *kinu B ‘garment’: The noun listed as a Class 2.4 noun in Kindaichi (1974) is not kinu ‘garment’ but kinu ‘silk.’ They both are also attested in OJ, the oldest attested stage of Japanese, and are generally recognized as cognates (Jodaigo 1967, Martin 1987). Their pitch patterns are both LH in MJ (Akinaga et al. 1997), a regular reflex of Class 2.4 in MJ. Additionally, there are modern Japanese dialects with kinu ‘garment’ also corresponding to Class 2.4 (Hirayama et al. eds. 1992–93). Reflexes of pR *kinu B ‘garment’ are observed throughout Ryukyu Islands. However, only a few dialects have nouns for ‘silk,’ in which the tonal and segmental correspondence is irregular (cf. Yamatohama k’iɴ ‘garment’ vs k’iɴ ‘garment’; Asama kiɴ BC ‘garment’ vs k’inuu A ‘silk’; Yoron kiɴ B ‘garment’ vs kinu B ‘silk’). It is suggested, therefore, that pR *kinu B lacked the meaning ‘silk’ and that apparently related Ryukyuan forms for ‘silk’ are Japanese loans. In any case, the fact that MJ and some Japanese dialects have kinu ‘garment’ belonging to Class 2.4 indicates that, regardless of whether ‘garment’ and ‘silk’ are cognates, the tone class of pR *kinu ‘garment’ is 2.4b.

38. *miso B ‘bean paste’: Matsumori (2012) reconstructs Class B. Tonal correspondence is regular between the dialects of Northern Ryukyuan but irregular between the dialects of Southern Ryukyuan. The former suggests that the pR Class of *miso is Class B, whereas the latter suggests it can be Class A, B, or C. The irregular tonal correspondence may suggest that borrowing is involved in this noun. However, the segmental correspondence is perfectly regular, which decreases the plausibility of the borrowing hypothesis. We, therefore, assume that the reflexes are inherited from pR, and the irregularity in the tonal correspondence is due to sporadic changes independently occurring in each dialect. Because Class B requires the minimal number of sound changes, we reconstruct Class B for pR *miso.

42. *soto B ‘outside’: Hattori (1979a,b) reconstructs Class B. In our primary data, reflexes of pR *soto exhibit a limited distribution. Hattori (1979a,b) reveals that its reflexes are observed in many dialects of Amami (in addition to Asama ɕutuu B, Kametsu ɕutu B, Shodon sɨt’uu BC, Naze sutʰu B, Aden sut’u B, and Onotsu sut’u BC). Because the tonal correspondences between dialects are regular and the reflexes are observed both in Amami and Okinawa, we assume that they were inherited from pR *soto ‘outside.’ The lack of its attestation in Southern Ryukyuan may be because its reflexes were replaced by almost synonymous pR *poka B ‘outside.’

49. *mado B ‘spare time’: Matsumori (2009, 2012) also reconstructs Class B for *mado ‘spare time,’ although she may not agree with the view that pR *mado ‘spare time’ is related to MJ Class 2.4 MJ mado ‘window.’ We postulate a metaphoric semantic change ‘window’ > ‘spare time,’ that is, the change from ‘special gap’ to ‘temporal gap.’ This type of change is also observed in other languages such as Russian (cf. oкнo ‘window,’ oкнo мeждy лeкциями ‘spare time between lectures’) (Morkovkin et al. [eds.] 2016, 678). The nouns for ‘window’ per se are scarcely attested in Ryukyuan. Even if they are, they tend to be unrelated to MJ mado 2.5, as in Shuri haɕiɾu BC ‘sliding window,’ takabaɕiɾu BC ‘high window,’ and Ishigaki takaɸusaɾa BC ‘high window.’ It is true that Taketomi and Yonaguni have mədu ‘window’ and Yonaguni madu B ‘window,’ respectively, but they may be a Japanese loan. The Yonaguni madu B ‘window’ recorded by Uwano (2013) is especially controversial, because, as pointed out by the author, the same speaker produces amadu A ‘window’ instead of madu B, as described in an earlier study (Uwano 2010). Thus, a plausible scenario is that at the stage of pR, *mado lost its original meaning of ‘window,’ whereas the form *mado itself survived with its meaning metaphorically changed from ‘window’ to ‘spare time.’

50. *majo B ‘eyebrow’: Although Martin (1987) regards MJ maju 2.5 ‘eyebrow’ and maju 2.5 ‘cocoon’ as cognates, they are listed as separate entries in the present study. Although the evidence for a distinction between pR *ju and *jo is in general scarce in modern Ryukyuan dialects (Pellard 2023), we reconstruct pR *majo, not *maju, based on the following evidence.[8] The final syllable of pR *majo B ‘eyebrow’ is reflected differently from that of pR *kaju A ‘porridge’ in many Ryukyuan dialects (Ashikebu majo AB ‘eyebrow’ vs kai AB ‘porridge’; Shuri maju B ‘eyebrow’ vs kee A ‘porridge’; Hatoma maju ‘eyebrow’ vs kai ‘porridge’; Ishigaki majoo BC ‘eyebrow’ vs kai A ‘porridge’). Although it still seems possible to reconstruct pR *kai, not *kaju, for ‘porridge,’ this hypothesis is rejected because the second syllable of pR *kaju ‘porridge’ yields different forms from that of pR *mai A ‘rice’ in at least two Yaeyama dialects, namely, Hateruma (kee ‘porridge’ vs mëë ‘rice’) and Aragusuku (kai ‘porridge’ vs majɨ ‘rice’). The correspondence with MJ kaju 2.1 ‘porridge’ also provides an additional support for the reconstruction of pR *kaju A ‘porridge.’

4.2 Nouns for which the pR class was newly reconstructed (23 words)

We newly reconstructed the pR tone classes of 23 nouns (Table 6). The majority of these nouns are attested in both Northern and Southern Ryukyuan, and their tonal correspondences are highly regular, but their pR tone classes have not been reconstructed in previous studies.

Table 6

Newly reconstructed Class 2.4a/2.5a and Class 2.4b/2.5b nouns

56. *kesa C ‘a short while ago’: We postulate a semantic change ‘this morning’ > ‘a short while ago’ following the NKDJ’s (2001, 1321) view. The Niigata dialect of Japanese also has kesa ‘a short while ago’ (Ohashi 2003, 90), suggesting that this type of semantic change is natural. Northern Ryukyuan forms exhibit an unexpected geminate, which requires further research.

58. *nau C ‘what’: For the reconstruction of *nau, not *nao, see Jarosz (2019).

59. *tubu C ‘grain’: Widespread Ryukyuan nouns for ‘grain’ are the reflexes of pR *tuzu C ‘grain’ (Yoron tɕiʑi C ‘id.,’ Ie sidzi AC ‘id.,’ Ishigaki tsɨdzɨ BC ‘id.,’ Yonaguni tɕ’iɴ C ). Evidence for *tubu ‘grain’ is sparse in the primary data, but in the secondary data, we find Yamatohama tz’ɨbu ‘grain,’ Taketomi subu ‘id.,’ and Ishigaki subu BC ‘grain.’ Tarama tsɨv C ‘dew’ discussed in Section 4.1 may at first glance seem to be a reflex of pR *tubu C ‘grain’; the difference in meaning between ‘grain’ and ‘dew’ appears to be the result of a contamination with pR *tuju C ‘dew.’ However, evidence shows that, although pR *bu is spirantized into v(u) in many environments in varieties of Miyako, including Tarama (cf. pR *kobu C ‘spider’ > Tarama kuvu C ‘id.,’ pR *abura B ‘oil’ > Tarama avva B ‘id.’), the spirantization does not occur when *bu is preceded by *tu. For example, in pR *tubusi A ‘knee’ (Yoron tɕinɕi A ‘id.,’ Nakijin tʑ’inɕi A ‘id.,’ Ishigaki tsɨbusɨ A ‘id.’) and pR *tuburu C ‘head, calabash’ (Yoron tɕibuɾu C ‘head,’ Nakijin tʑ’imbu C ‘id.,’ Ishigaki tsɨburɨ BC ‘id.’), *bu is not spirantized in Tarama as in tsɨbusɨ A ‘knee’ and tsɨbuɭ C ‘calabash.’ In other varieties of Miyako, *bu in these nouns is reflected as ɡu (Irabu tsɨɡʊsɨ ‘knee,’ tsɨɡʊɨ ‘calabash’; Ikema sɨɡusɨ A ‘knee,’ tsɨɡui C ‘calabash’) (Celik 2020), likely through sporadic change[9]. Therefore, Tarama tsɨv C ‘dew’ is not a reflex of pR *tubu C ‘grain.’ This evidence also indicates that Ikema tubu C ‘grain’ is a regular reflex of pR *tubu C ‘id.’

61. *tuti C ‘hammer’: In the primary data, its reflexes are only attested in four dialects of North Ryukyuan, but in the secondary data, they are also attested in Southern Ryukyuan (Ishigaki tsɨtsɨ BC ‘hammer’). Therefore, it is safe to assume that this noun exists in pR.

71. *puna B ‘gibel’: As far as the data in Table 6 are concerned, the principle of parsimony cannot determine whether the tone class of pR *puna ‘gibel’ is B or C. The cognates of pR *puna are infrequent in Ryukyuan dialects. However, in Uwano (1998) study on many varieties in Okinoerabu Island, 24 of the 35 varieties have the cognates of pR *puna, and in all but one of the 24 varieties, the pitch pattern corresponds to Class B. Based on this, we reconstruct pR *puna B ‘gibel.’ The segmental correspondence in the first syllable in Yonaguni huna is irregular (compare pR *puna ‘gibel’ with pR *pune C ‘boat’ [Yonaguni nni C, Yoron puni C, Shuri ɸuɲi B] and with pR *pugori A ‘testicles’ [Yonaguni ŋɡui A, Yoron pugui A, Shuri ɸuɡui A]). It is therefore possible that borrowing is involved in this noun for Yonaguni. If Yonaguni huna C corresponding to Class C is a loanword, then the view that the tone class of pR *puna ‘gibel’ is B becomes more plausible.

73. *koto B ‘zither’: The segmental correspondences are irregular, suggesting that borrowing is involved. The correspondence between MJ and Southern Ryukyuan suggests that the first syllable in pR is *ko. Although pR *ko reflects as ɸu in Ie and Nakijin, the first syllable in ‘zither’ is kʰu in both dialects. The Shuri form has an unexpected long vowel in the second syllable. The Asama form has an unexpected vowel o, and the speaker is aware that this is a new word (Uwano 2017a, 152). However, the regularity of tonal correspondence (except Yonaguni) and the widespread attestation make us hesitant to deny the existence of this noun in pR.

74. *majo B ‘cocoon’: It is more difficult to determine whether the final syllable of ‘eyebrow’ is *ju or *jo than in the case of pR *majo B ‘eyebrow.’ Ashikebu distinguishes between majo AB ‘eyebrow’ and mai AB ‘cocoon’ and the final vowel of the latter agrees with that of kai AB ‘porridge’ (<pR *kaju A ‘id.’); therefore, pR *maju ‘eyebrow’ is favored. However, Taketomi, which does not distinguish məju BC ‘eyebrow’ and məju BC ‘cocoon,’ has kəi A ‘porridge’ with a different vowel from that in ‘eyebrow’ and ‘cocoon,’ supporting pR *majo B ‘cocoon.’ Although the conflicting sound correspondences are difficult to interpret, we reconstruct pR *majo B ‘cocoon’ first, because both ‘eyebrow’ and ‘cocoon’ are majo 1 in OJ, and second, because there is a theory (Martin 1987, 474) that views these two nouns as cognates.

4.3 Class 2.4–2.5 nouns whose pR classes remain unsettled

The pR tone classes of the 17 nouns shown in Table 7 remain unsettled primarily because of irregular correspondences between Ryukyuan dialects or between pR and MJ.

Table 7

Class 2.4–2.5 nouns for which pR tone classes remain unsettled. Ha79, Hattori (1979a,b), Ma09, Matsumori (2009), Bo10, de Boer (2010)

76. *itu A? ‘when,’ 77. *sumi A? ‘inside corner,’ 78. *abo A? ‘horsefly,’ 79. *aki A? ‘autumn,’ 80. *pabu A? ‘snake,’ 81. *turo A? ‘crane’: The comparison of Ryukyuan dialects suggests that the tone class of these six nouns is Class A, which is an irregular reflex of MJ Class 2.4–2.5. At least for some of them, the irregularity may be explained by borrowing. However, the relatively regular tonal correspondences between dialects suggest that the borrowing, if any, occurred at the stage of pR. We leave open the issue regarding the irregularity and the reconstruction of tone classes of these nouns.

For *pabu A ‘snake,’ there is an additional issue about its cognacy. The irregularity of correspondence between MJ femi 2.5 ‘snake’ and pR *pabu A occurs not only in tones but also in segments. Furthermore, the cognates of pR *pabu A usually refer to a venomous snake endemic to the Ryukyu Islands, rather than to snakes in general; thus, the meanings do not exactly correspond. Therefore, instead of the view that pR *pabu A is related to MJ femi 2.5 ‘snake’ (e.g., Jarosz et al. [2022]), we should explore the possibility that pR *pabu A is related to MJ fami ‘viper.’ Cognates that may be related to MJ fami ‘viper’ are widely distributed in Kansai, Chugoku, and Shikoku Districts in the forms of hami, hame, hamu, habi, habe, habu, and so forth, all of which mean ‘viper’ (LAJ 19661974, Map 228). Crucially, the pitch pattern of MJ fami ‘viper’ is HL, a regular reflex of Class 2.2, which in turn regularly corresponds to pR Class A. Meanwhile, Wadomari and Shuri have nouns referring to ‘snake’ in general, namely, çibu C and ɸiibu C, respectively. Although evidence is sparse, it may be possible to reconstruct pR *pebu C ‘snake’ along with pR *pabu A ‘venomous snake.’ If this holds true, then it is pR *pebu C ‘snake’ that is related to MJ femi ‘snake,’ and its tonal subclass is Class 2.5a.

82. *aka A? ‘red,’ 83. *ao B? ‘blue,’ 84. *kuro B? ‘black,’ 85. *siro B? ‘white’: These four words are color terms, which do not appear as free nominal morphemes in Ryukyuan but are adjectives that appear with a following component. In Table 7, the adjectives followed by a noun for ‘color’ (a reflex of pR *ero B ‘color’ [Pellard 2013]) are represented. *aka ‘red’ seems to belong to pR Class A, whereas the others seem to belong to pR Class B. However, these classes are those of adjectives, not nouns. Adjectives may have different tone systems than nouns, and the pR tone system of adjectives has been little investigated. We therefore do not reconstruct the tone class of these color terms.

86. *kazu B∼C ‘number’: Although de Boer (2010) reconstructs Class C, half of the dialects in our data exhibit pitch patterns corresponding with Class B.

90. *gaki?? ‘oyster’: Hattori (1979a,b) regards pR as having a cognate whose tone class is Class B based on five Northern Ryukyuan dialects: Asama gatsɨɨtsɨ C ‘oyster?,’ Nakijin gatʑii B ‘oyster’ (gatʑ’ii B, Nakasone 1983), Kushi gakii BC ‘id.,’ Onna gaatɕi B ‘id.,’ and Shuri ɡatɕitɕaa BC ‘sea urchin.’ However, the meaning of Asama gatsɨɨtsɨ C is not ‘oyster’ but ‘urchin’ according to Uwano (2017a, 157), and the meaning of Shuri ɡatɕitɕaa BC is, as Hattori (1979a,b) also noticed, ‘urchin.’ In terms of segmental correspondence, too, these two nouns appear unrelated to MJ kaki 2.5 ‘oyster.’ The dialects listed in Hattori (1979a,b) that include the nouns straightforwardly related to MJ kaki are Nakijin, Kushi, and Onna. Yamatohama also has gaki ‘oyster.’ Therefore, it is possible to reconstruct pR *gaki B ‘oyster,’ although its reflexes are observed only in Northern Ryukyuan.

However, if we follow Hattori’s theory that ‘oyster’ and ‘urchin’ are related, it is necessary to compare the nouns for ‘urchin’ between dialects. They have various reflexes, such as Asama gatsɨɨtsɨ C, Ie ɡasisi AC, Nakijin ɡaɕiiɕi C, Shuri ɡatɕitɕaa BC, Tarama kadzɨtsɨ C, Ikema kazɨtsɨ C, Ishigaki kakidzɨ BC, and Taketomi kəkitɕi. Their tonal correspondences are perfectly regular, which allows us to reconstruct them as belonging to Class C. Irregular segmental correspondences make it difficult to reconstruct the proto-form. Assuming that variation in voicing across dialects is due to a sporadic voicing of original voiceless consonants, the proto-forms can be reconstructed as *kakisu, *kakiti, *kakesu, *kaketi, *kasesu, *kaseti, and so forth. If the proto-form for “urchin” were *kasesu or *kaseti, then it would be related not with MJ kaki ‘oyster’ but with MJ kase ‘urchin.’ By contrast, if the proto-form were *kakiti or *kakisu, then hypothetical relatedness between ‘oyster’ and ‘urchin’ becomes more plausible. In fact, Ashikebu has gak’isɨ B ‘oyster’ (not ‘urchin’!), which corresponds to *kakisu. The Ashikebu form would provide a support for Hattori’s (1979a,b) theory that the nouns for ‘oyster’ and ‘urchin’ are related. Noteworthy is that the pR tone class of ‘urchin’ is reconstructed as Class C based on the regular tonal correspondence between many dialects, whereas we reconstructed Class B for *gaki ‘oyster’ based on limited data of Northern Ryukyuan. If we focus on the limited distribution of *gaki, we can regard it as a loanword at later stage and reconstruct pR *kaki(-su) ‘oyster,’ whose tone class is C. The history of the pR noun for ‘oyster’ is complicated, which forces us to reserve judgment as to whether its tone class is B or C.

91. *koi?? ‘carp’: Only two dialects had a reflex of this noun in our primary data. Asama koi is irregular, as the dialect experienced a change from pR *ko to ku in word-initial position. In addition, it is a neologism according to the source. Irabu and Taketomi have kʊi and kui, respectively, although information about pitch patterns is not available. Nakijin and Shuri have polymorphemic forms, kʰuuʔjuu B and kuuʔiju BC, respectively. The second components must be a reflex of pR *io A ‘fish,’ although it is not clear whether the first component is related to MJ kofi 2.5 ‘carp.’ It is especially doubtful for Nakijin, where pR *ko changes into ɸu in a word-initial position. By contrast, Ishigaki has a more transparent form kuiʔidzu BC?. A relatively reliable datum for identifying the pR tone class of *koi is Tarama kui C; however, only one reflex is insufficient for reconstructing the tone class.

93. * asa- ? ‘morning’: Although the presence of *asa ‘morning’ in pR has been doubted (Vovin 2008, 2010), as Pellard (2017) argues, *asa- is considered to exist in pR because the cognates of MJ asa 2.5 ‘morning’ are observed in many compounds in both Northern and Southern Ryukyuan. The compounds containing pR *asa- include Yamatohama ʔasa-duɾɨ ‘morning calm’ and ʔasa-tz’ɨju ‘morning dew’; Yoron asa-ɕu ‘morning tide’; Ie ʔasa-duɾi ‘morning calm,’ asa-ɕu ‘morning tide,’ and asa-tida ‘morning sun’; Nakijin hasaa-duɾii ‘morning calm’ and hasaa-tz’iju ‘morning dew’; Shuri ʔasa-duɾi ‘morning calm’ and asa-tsiju ‘morning dew’; Tarama aɕa-duri ‘morning calm,’ aɕa-tsɿv ‘morning dew,’ and aɕa-ɕu ‘morning tide’; Hatoma ʔasa-duri ‘morning calm,’ ʔasa-pana ‘early morning,’ and ʔasa-kai ‘morning porridge’; and Ishigaki ʔasa-duri ‘morning calm’ and ʔasa-kai ‘morning porridge.’ It is unclear whether pR had a free morpheme *asa ‘morning’ because its reflexes are rarely, or not at all, used in isolation. Because the method of determining the tone class of the constituents of a compound is only established for a limited number of dialects, we do not reconstruct the tone class of the bound morpheme *asa- ‘morning.’

4.4 Class 2.4–2.5 nouns whose existence in pR is doubtful

A total of 15 MJ Class 2.4 nouns shown in Table 8 have not been reconstructed for pR, because their existence in pR is strongly doubted.

Table 8

MJ Class 2.4–2.5 nouns that may not exist in pR

94. ama 2.4 “nun” 95. geta 2.4 “clogs” 96. iti 2.4 “market” 97. kai 2.4 “oar”
98. kine 2.4 “pestle” 99. kiri 2.4 “awl” 100. titi 2.4 “milk” 101. titi 2.4 “father”
102. tuba 2.4 “sword guard” 103. tuwe 2.4 “cane” 104. aju 2.5 “sweet fish” 105. ani 2.5 “brother”
106. famo 2.5 “sea eel” 107. sake 2.5 “salmon” 108. wido 2.5 “well”

95. geta 2.4 ‘clogs’: Although we find Yamatohama gɜtʰa ‘clogs’ and Shuri dʑita B ‘id.,’ they must be borrowings. First, the segmental correspondence between Shuri and MJ is irregular. Second, the two dialects also have synonymous ʔaɕidʑa ‘clogs’ and ʔaɕidʑa BC ‘id.,’ respectively. They are reflexes of pR *asida B∼C ‘id.,’ whose reflexes are widely observed in Ryukyuan dialects.

96. iti 2.4 ‘market’: We observe only Yamatohama itʑ’i ‘market.’ The limited attestation suggests that it is a Japanese loan.

97. kai 2.4 ‘oar’: Only Shuri has a form, kee BC ‘oar,’ that may be related to MJ kai 2.4 ‘oar.’ However, Shuri also has a synonymous ʔjeeku C ‘oar,’ which is a reflex of pR *ijako B∼C ‘oar.’ The reflexes of *ijako are observed throughout Ryukyu Islands (Yoron jahu B, Ie ʔjaaɸu AC, Nakijin jaɸuu B, Tarama ɨzaku C, Ikema zzaku B, Yonaguni daŋŋu B). Shuri kee BC ‘oar’ can therefore be regarded as a borrowing.

99. kiri 2.4 ‘awl’: We observe Yoron ii B ‘awl,’ Nakijin ʔiɾii B ‘id.,’ Tarama B ‘id.,’ Yonaguni iri B ‘id.,’ and so forth, which allow the reconstruction of pR *iri B ‘awl.’ Although pR *iri B ‘awl’ at first glance seems related to MJ kiri 2.4 ‘id.,’ the resemblance must be superficial. Based on Igarashi (2021), we assume that pR *iri B ‘awl’ is instead related to a verb igir- ‘to make a hole with an awl’ observed in modern Japanese dialects spoken in Kyushu. Igarashi’s (2021) hypothesis is motivated by a geographical distribution of various nouns for ‘awl’ in Kyushu. Japanese dialects spoken in Kagoshima and Miyazaki Prefectures have reflexes of *iri, not *kiri (NHDJ 1989, 195, Hashiguchi 2004, 138–39, 211). In addition, reflexes of *igiri ‘awl’ are observed in dialects spoken in Izumi and Akune, the northernmost cities in Kagoshima Prefecture, as well as dialects in Kumamoto and Nagasaki Prefectures, located north of Kagoshima Prefecture (NHDJ 1989, 155, Hashiguchi 2004, 148). Because *iri and *igiri complement one another geographically and their meanings coincide, they must be cognate. The former must have developed from the latter though an irregular sound change that drops the intervocalic *g. Notably, a verb igir- ‘to make a hole with an awl’ is attested in dialects spoken in Kumamoto, Nagasaki, Saga, and Fukuoka Prefectures (NHDJ 1989, 155). This region completely covers the places where reflexes of *igiri are attested, suggesting that *igiri is a gerund of a verb *igir-. These observations lead us to assume that pR*iri is related to *iri, *igiri, and *igir-, but not to MJ kiri ‘awl.’ The irregular dropping of the intervocalic *g in *igiri is considered as a shared innovation between Ryukyuan and Japanese dialects in south Kyushu.

100. titi 2.4 ‘milk’: Yoron tɕiitɕii C ‘milk,’ Nakijin tʑ’iitʑ’ii C, and Shuri tɕiitɕii BC seems related to titi 2.4 ‘milk.’ However, they all are nursery words. The pR form for ‘milk’ is *tii B (cf. Ie tɕ’ii B, Nakijin tʑ’ii B, Shuri tɕii B, Tarama tsɨɨ B, Ikema tsɨɨ B). The three nursery words must be a reduplication of pR *tii B ‘milk’ that developed independently in each dialect.

102. tuba 2.4 ‘sword guard’: We observe only one attestation in Yamatohama tz’ɨba ‘sword guard,’ which can be considered a borrowing.

103. tuwe 2.4 ‘cane’: We find Tarama dii A ‘handle’ and Ikema dii C ‘id.’ that seem related to MJ tuwe 2.4 ‘cane.’ However, such apparently cognate nouns are attested only in Miyako, and their segmental correspondence with MJ is irregular. Moreover, the unexplained semantic change ‘cane’ > ‘handle’ (or ‘handle’ > ‘cane’) makes the hypothetical cognacy doubtful.

107. sake 2.5 ‘salmon’: We only observe Yamatohama sakʰɜ ‘salmon,’ which is considered a Japanese loan.

The other nouns listed in Table 8 have not been attested in our sources.

5 Discussion

Our survey reconstructed the pR tone classes of a total of 75 Class 2.4–2.5 nouns. They are roughly split in half between Class C (i.e., Class 2.4a/2.5a; 36 nouns) and Class B (i.e., Class 2.4b/2.5b; 39 nouns). The tonal correspondence between Ryukyuan dialects is generally regular, confirming that the split correspondence is genuine.

Now that our survey has expanded the inventory of pR tone classes by 20 words, we can test the hypothesis that the split correspondence is due to a conditioned merger (Kindaichi 1960, Hirayama et al. 1966, Tokugawa 1990) based on a larger range of data. The conditioned merger hypothesis states that Class 2.4–2.5 nouns ending in a non-high vowel were tonally merged into Class 2.3 (corresponding to pR Class B), whereas those ending in a high vowel remained as a distinct class (i.e., pR Class C). Table 9 provides a contingency table that shows the cross tabulation of pR Class with word-final vowel (non-high vs high) in Class 2.4–2.5 nouns. Table 10 shows which nouns belong to each cell of Table 9, with a distinction between Classes 2.4 and 2.5. Nouns that contradict the predictions of the conditioned merger hypothesis are shaded.

Table 9

The cross tabulation of pR Class with word-final vowel (non-high vs high) in Class 2.4–2.5 nouns

Table 10

Classification of Class 2.4–2.5 nouns according to their tone classes in pR and word-final vowels

Although there is a strong tendency for nouns with a word-final high vowel not to belong to 2.4b/2.5b (Class B), 10 of the 31 nouns go against this tendency. The tendency for nouns with a final non-high vowel not to belong to 2.4a/2.5a (Class C) is slightly weaker; 15 of the 44 nouns do not obey this tendency. Therefore, the conditioned merger hypothesis has also not been supported by this study. The absence of clear conditioning factors causing the split correspondence between MJ and pR requires reconstructing more contrasts for pJ.

However, although the conditioned merger hypothesis is not tenable in its original formulation, it is also true that the skewed distribution in Table 9 strongly suggests a conditioned merger. Indeed, the association between the word-final vowel (non-high vs high) and the tone classes is statistically significant according to Pearson’s chi-square test with Yates’ continuity correction (χ 2 (1) = 6.9578, p < 0.01), although the strength of the association is moderate (Cramer’s V = 0.3317 (95% CI [0.1132, 0.5195]). Observing the results of an analysis similar to that of this subsection (based on 42–43 nouns), de Boer (2010, 232) maintains that “[i]t is therefore not impossible that the presence of a close vowel in the second syllable played some role in preventing the merger of members of class 2.4/5 with class 2.3.” On the one hand, it seems premature to reject the conditioned merger hypothesis completely, but on the other hand, to defend the hypothesis, we need to revise it so that it can account for exceptions. One solution would be to assume that another factor besides word-final high vowel prevented the merger. However, such assumption is merely speculative without further investigation. At the moment, the distinction between Class 2.4a/2.5a and Class 2.4b/2.5b must be seen as tracing back to pJ.

6 Conclusion

Although it is generally accepted that MJ Class 2.4–2.5 exhibits a split correspondence with pR tone classes, the exact membership of pR tone classes has not been fully elucidated. Based on the comparison of a larger range of data of both Northern and Southern Ryukyuan, this study reconstructed the pR tone classes of 75 Class 2.4–2.5 nouns. Class 2.4–2.5 nouns were roughly split in half between pR Class C and Class B. The tonal correspondence between Ryukyuan dialects was generally regular, confirming that the split correspondence was genuine, not merely a sporadic irregularity. A hypothesis that the split correspondence is due to a conditioned merger was also examined, and no reliable conditioning factors accounting for the split correspondence were observed, even based on a wider range of data. Therefore, unless future research would identify the factors causing the split, we must regard the distinction between 2.4a/2.5a and 2.4b/2.5b as an archaic feature tracing back to pJ, which requires reconstructing additional contrasts for pJ. The results of this study will advance a more thorough investigation into the reconstruction of the pR and pJ tone systems.

Abbreviations

Am

Amami

Bo10

de Boer (2010)

Ha79

Hattori (1979a,b)

Ma09

Matsumori (2009)

Ma12

Matsumori (2012)

Mi

Miyako

MJ

Middle Japanese

OJ

Old Japanese

Ok

Okinawa

pJ

proto-Japonic

pR

proto-Ryukyuan

Sh07

Shimabukuro (2007)

Yo

Yonaguni

Acknowledgment

I would like to thank Kenan Celik and Kohei Nakazawa for helpful discussions.

  1. Funding information: This work was supported by JSPS KAKENHI Grant Numbers 17H02332, 19H00530, 16H01933, and 21K00517, as well as NINJAL projects of “Cross-linguistic Studies of Japanese Prosody and Grammar,” “Endangered Languages and Dialects in Japan,” and “Empirical Study on the Intonational Diversity in Japanese and Ryukyuan Dialects.”

  2. Conflict of interest: The author states no conflict of interest.

  3. Data availability statement: All data generated or analyzed during this study are included in this published article.

References

Akinaga, Kazue, Kazuaki Ueno, Kiyoe Sakamoto, Eisaku Sato, and Yutaka Suzuki. 1997. Nihongo Akusentoshi Sōgō Shiryō: Sakuinhen. Tokyo: Tokyodo Shuppan.Search in Google Scholar

ASB 1959 = Kyugakkai Rengo Amami Oshima Kyodo Chosa Iinkai. (ed.), 1959. Amami: Shizen to Bunka. Tokyo: Nihon Gakujutu Shinkokai.Search in Google Scholar

de Boer, Elisabeth. 2010. The historical development of Japanese tone. Wiesbaden: Harrassowitz.Search in Google Scholar

Celik, Kenan. 2020. Minami Ryūkyū Miyakogoshi. Doctoral dissertation. Kyoto University.Search in Google Scholar

Hashiguchi, Mitsuru. 2004. Kagoshima Hōgen Daijiten. Volume 1. Kagoshima: Taki Shobo.Search in Google Scholar

Hattori, Shiro. 1958. “Amami guntō no shohōgen ni tsuite: Okinawa, Sakishima shohōgen to no hikaku.” Jinrui Kagaku 11, 79–99.Search in Google Scholar

Hattori, Shiro. 1979a. “Nihon sogo ni tsuite 21.” Gekkan Gengo 8(11), 97–107.Search in Google Scholar

Hattori, Shiro. 1979b. “Nihon sogo ni tsuite 22.” Gekkan Gengo 8(12), 100–14.Search in Google Scholar

Hirayama, Teruo, Ichiro Oshima, and Masachie Nakamoto. 1966. Ryūkyū Hōgen no Sōgōteki Kenkyū. Tokyo: Meiji Shoin.Search in Google Scholar

Hirayama, Teruo, Ichiro Oshima, and Masachie Nakamoto. 1967. Ryūkyū Sakishima Hōgen no Sōgōteki Kenkyū. Tokyo: Meiji Shoin.Search in Google Scholar

Hirayama, Teruo. 1988. Minami Ryūkyū no Hōgen Kiso Goi. Tokyo: Ofusha.Search in Google Scholar

Hirayama, Teruo, and Masachie Nakamoto. 1964. Ryūkyū Yonaguni Hōgen no Kenkyū. Tokyo: Tokyodo.Search in Google Scholar

Hirayama, Teruo, Ono Makio, Mariko Kuno, Ichiro Oshima, Kuno Makoto, and Sugimura Takao. (eds.). 1992–1993. Gendai Nihongo Hōgen Daijiten 1–6. Tokyo: Meiji Shoin.Search in Google Scholar

Igarashi, Yosuke. 2016a. “Minami Ryūkyū Miyakogo Ikema hōgen Tarama hōgen no inritsu kōzō.” Gengo Kenkyū 150, 33–57.Search in Google Scholar

Igarashi, Yosuke. 2016b. “Meishi no imi ga kakawaru akusento no gōryū: Minami Ryūkyū Miyakogo Ikema hōgen no jirei.” Journal of the Phonetic Society of Japan 20(3), 46–65.Search in Google Scholar

Igarashi, Yosuke. 2016c. “Akusentogata no taiō ni motozuite Nichiryūsogo wo saiken suru tame no goi risuto Nichiryūgo Ryuibetsugoi.” Proceedings of the 2016 Spring meeting of the Society for Japanese Linguistics. Tokyo, Japan: Gakushuin University.Search in Google Scholar

Igarashi, Yosuke. 2018. “3-paku meishi dai 4-rui ni okeru Hondo Nihongo to Ryūkyūgo kan no 1 tai 2 no akusentogata no taiō ni tsuite.” Paper presented at NINJAL Project Meeting “Cross-linguistic Studies of Japanese Prosody and Grammar.” Okinawa, Japan: University of the Ryukyus.Search in Google Scholar

Igarashi, Yosuke. 2021. “Bunkigakuteki shuhō ni motozuita Nichiryū sogo no keitō bunrui no kokoromi.” In Fīrudo to Bunken kara Miru Nichiryū Sogo no Keitō to Rekishi edited by Nobuko Kibe, Yuka Hayashi, and Tomohide Kinuhata, 17–51. Tokyo: Kaitakusha.Search in Google Scholar

Igarashi, Yosuke, Yukinori Takubo, Yuka Hayashi, and Tomoyuki Kubo. 2011. “How many tonal contrasts in Ikema Ryukyuan?” Proceedings of the 17th International Congress of Phonetic Sciences, August 17–21, 2011, Hong Kong, China, 930–33.Search in Google Scholar

Igarashi, Yosuke, Yukinori Takubo, Yuka Hayashi, Thomas Pellard, Tomoyuki Kubo. 2012. “Ryūkyū Miyakogo Ikema hōgen no akusento taikei wa 3-kei de atte 2-kei de wa nai.” Journal of the Phonetic Society of Japan 16(1), 134–48.Search in Google Scholar

Igarashi, Yosuke, Yukinori Takubo, Yuka Hayashi, Tomoyuki Kubo. 2018. “Tonal neutralization in the Ikema dialect of Miyako Ryukyuan.” In Tonal Change and Neutralization edited by Haruo Kubozono and Mikio Giriko, 81–128. Berlin: Mouton De Gruyter.10.1515/9783110567502-005Search in Google Scholar

Jarosz, Aleksandra. 2019. “Fine details of a larger picture: Proto-Sakishima *au and *ao diphthongs.” Folia Linguistica Historica 40(2), 401–37.10.1515/flih-2019-0016Search in Google Scholar

Jarosz, Aleksandra, Martine Robbeets, Ricardo Fernandes, Hiroto Takamiya, Akito Shinzato, Naoko Nakamura, Maria Shinoto, and Mark Hudson. 2022. “Demography, trade and state power: A tripartite model of medieval farming/language dispersals in the Ryukyu Islands.” Evolutionary Human Sciences 4(E4), 1–22.10.1017/ehs.2022.1Search in Google Scholar

Jodaigo, Jiten Henshu Iinkai (ed.). 1967. Jidaibetsu kokugo daijiten: Jōdaihen. Tokyo: Sanseido.Search in Google Scholar

Kajiku, Shin’ichi. 2020. Hatoma Hōgen Jiten, edited by Natsuko Nakagawa. Tokyo: The National Institute for Japanese Language and Linguistics.Search in Google Scholar

Kiku, Chiyo and Toshizō Takahashi. 2005. Yoron Hōgen Jiten. Tokyo: Musashino Shoin.Search in Google Scholar

Kindaichi, Haruhiko. 1960. Akusento kara mita Ryūkyūgo shohōgen no keitō. Tōkyō Gaikokugo Daigaku Ronshū 7, 59–80.Search in Google Scholar

Kindaichi, Haruhiko. 1974. Kokugo Akusento no Shiteki Kenkyū: Genri to Hōhō. Tokyo: Hanawa Shobo.Search in Google Scholar

Kuno, Mariko. 1991. “Okinoerabujima Wadomari hōgen no akusento: Nagasa ga akusento no kata wo shimesu hōgen.” In Oshima Ichiro Kyoju Taikan Kinen Ronshu Kankokai, edited by Nihongo Ronkō. Tokyo: Ofusha.Search in Google Scholar

Maeara, Toru, Eikichi Hateruma, Hoyu Takamine, Teruo Irisato. 2011. Taketomi Hōgen Jiten. Okinawa: Nanzansha.Search in Google Scholar

Martin, Samuel E. 1987. The Japanese Language through time. London: Yale University Press.Search in Google Scholar

Matsumori, Akiko. 1995. “Kakōshiki akusento no yurai to Shikoku tōhokubu shohōgen no keitō.” Tokyo University Linguistic Papers 14, 557–81.Search in Google Scholar

Matsumori, Akiko. 1997. “Tokushimaken Wakimachi Mikamochō no akusento to Hondosogo no akusento taikei.” Kokugogaku 189, 55–68.Search in Google Scholar

Matsumori, Akiko. 1998. “Ryūkyū akusento no rekishiteki keisei katei: Ruibetsu goi 2-hakugo no tokui na gōryū no shikata wo tegakari ni.” Gengo Kenkyū 114, 85–114.Search in Google Scholar

Matsumori, Akiko. 2000a. “Ryūkyū no takei akusento taikei ni tsuite no ichi kōsatsu: Ryūkyū sogo ni okeru ruibetsu goi 3-pakugo no gōryū no shikata.” Kokugogaku 51(1), 93–108.Search in Google Scholar

Matsumori, Akiko. 2000b. “Ryūkyū akusento chōsa no tame no ruibetsugoi no kaihatsu: Okinoerabu jima no chōsa kara.” Journal of Phonetic Society of Japan 4(1), 61–71.Search in Google Scholar

Matsumori, Akiko. 2009. “Okinawa hontō Kin hōgen no akusento gata to sono keiretsu: Ryūkyū chōsayō keiretsubetsu goi.” Memoirs of the Japan Women’s University, Faculty of Literature 58, 97–122.Search in Google Scholar

Matsumori, Akiko. 2010. “Taramajima no 3-kei akusento to keiretsubetsu goi.” Nihongo Kenkyū 12-shō edited by Zendo Uwano, 490–503. Tokyo: Meiji Shoin.Search in Google Scholar

Matsumori, Akiko. 2012. “Ryūkyūgo chōsayō keiretsubetsu goi no soan.” Journal of Phonetic Society of Japan 16(1), 30–40.Search in Google Scholar

Matsumori, Akiko. 2013. “Miyakojima ni okeru 3-kei akusento taikei no hakken: Yonaha hōgen no baai.” NINJAL Research Paper 6, 67–92.Search in Google Scholar

Matsumori, Akiko. 2015. “Minami Ryūkyū no 3-kei akusento: sono inritsu tan’i ni kansuru kōsatsu.” Memoirs of the Japan Women’s University, Faculty of Literature 64, 55–92.Search in Google Scholar

Matsumori, Akiko. 2016. “Yaeyama shotō Kuroshima hōgen akusento no shikumi: sono inritsu hanchū PWd to sagarime no shutsugen jōken.” Gengo Kenkyū 150, 59–85.Search in Google Scholar

Miyagi, Shin’yu. 2003. Ishigaki Hōgen Jiten. Okinawa: The Okinawa Times.Search in Google Scholar

Miyanaga, Masamori. 1930. Yaeyama Goi. Tokyo: Toyo Bunko Sokan.Search in Google Scholar

Morkovkin, V. V., G. F. Bogačëva, and N. M. Luckaja eds. 2016. Bol’šoj Universal’nyj Slovar’ Russkogo Jazyka. Moskow: Slovari XXI veka, AST-PRESS ŠKOLA.Search in Google Scholar

Nakasone, Seizen. 1983. Okinawa Nakijin Hōgen Jiten. Tokyo: Kadokawa Gakugei Shuppan.Search in Google Scholar

NKDJ 2001. = Nihon Kokugo Daijiten Henshu Iinkai. (ed.). 2001. Nihon Kokugo Daijiten, volume 2, 2nd edition. Tokyo: Shogakkan.Search in Google Scholar

NHDJ 1989. = Shōgakkan Kokugo Jiten Henshūbu (ed.). 1989. Nihon Hōgen Daijiten, volume 3. Tokyo: Shogakkan.Search in Google Scholar

LAJ 1966–1974. = National Institute for Japanese Language and Linguistics (ed.). 1966–1974. Linguistic Atlas of Japan. Tokyo: The Printing Bureau, Ministry of Finance.Search in Google Scholar

Ohashi, Katsuo. 2003. Niigata Hōgen Jiten. Tokyo: Ofusha.Search in Google Scholar

OKJ 1963. = Kokuritsu kokugo kenkyūjo (ed.) 1963/2001. Okinawago Jiten. Tokyo: The Printing Bureau, Ministry of Finance.Search in Google Scholar

Osada, Suma, Nahoko Suyama, and Misako Fujii. 1980. Amami Hōgen Bunrui Jiten, volume 2. Tokyo: Kasama Shoin.Search in Google Scholar

Oshio, Mutsuko. 2009. Okinawa Iejima Hōgen Jiten, 2nd edition. Okinawa: Ieson Kyoiku Iinkai.Search in Google Scholar

Pellard, Thomas. 2009a. Ogami: Éléments de description d’un parler du Sud des Ryukyu. Ph.D. dissertation, École des hautes études en sciences sociales.Search in Google Scholar

Pellard, Thomas. 2009b. “Once again on long vowels and register in Proto-Japonic.” Presented at International Workshop on “The history & reconstruction of Japanese accent”, 3–4 September 2009, Paris, France.Search in Google Scholar

Pellard, Thomas. 2013. “Ryukyuan perspectives on the proto-Japonic vowel system.” In Japanese/Korean Linguistics 20 edited by Bjarke Frellesvig and Peter Sells, 81–96. Standford: CSLI Publishers.Search in Google Scholar

Pellard, Thomas. 2015. “The linguistic archeology of the Ryukyu Islands.” In Handbook of the Ryukyuan Languages: History, Structure, and Use edited by Patrick Heinrich, Shinsho Miyara, Michinori Shimoji, 13–37. Berlin: De Gruyter Mouton.10.1515/9781614511151.13Search in Google Scholar

Pellard, Thomas. 2017. “A (more) comparative approach to some Japanese etymologies.” In Studies in Japanese and Korean Historical and Theoretical Linguistics and Beyond edited by William McClure, Alexander Vovin, 56–65. Leiden: Brill.10.1163/9789004351134_007Search in Google Scholar

Pellard, Thomas. 2023. “Ryukyuan and the reconstruction of proto-Japanese-Ryukyuan.” To be published in Handbook of Japanese Historical Linguistics edited by Bjarke Frellesvig, Satoshi Kinsui and John Whitman. De Gruyter Mouton.Search in Google Scholar

Shimabukuro, Moriyo. 2007. A reconstruction of the Accentual History of the Japanese and Ryukyuan Languages. Folkstone: Global Oriental.10.1163/ej.9781901903638.i-376Search in Google Scholar

Shimabukuro, Moriyo. 2008. “A reconstruction of proto-Japanese accent for disyllabic nouns: Focusing on the problem of subclasses.” Proto-Japanese: Issues and Prospects edited by Bjarke Frellesvig, John Whitman, 125–139. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.10.1075/cilt.294.10shiSearch in Google Scholar

Takayama, Michiaki. 2003. “Jion seichō to Nihongo no akusento.” Kokugogaku 54(3), 16–29.Search in Google Scholar

Tokugawa, Munemasa. 1990. “Nihon no hōgen: Nihongo no keisei to no kakawari.” In Nihongo no Keisei edited by Osamu Sakiyama. Tokyo: SanseidoSearch in Google Scholar

Tokuyama, Shun’ei, and Kenan Celik. 2020. Minami Ryūkyū Miyakogo Tarama Hōgen Jiten. Tokyo: National Institute for Japanese Language and Linguistics.Search in Google Scholar

Tomihama, Sadakichi. 2013. Miyako Irabu Hōgen Jiten. Okinawa: The Okinawa Times.Search in Google Scholar

Uemura, Yukio. 1977. “Ryūkyū rettō no gengo: Sōran.” In Nihon Rettō no Gengo edited by Takashi Kamei, Rokuro Kono, and Eiichi Chino, 311–354. Tokyo: Sanseido.Search in Google Scholar

Uwano, Zendo. 1977. “Tokunoshima Asama hōgen no akusento (1).” In Komatsushiro Yūichi Kyōju Taishoku, Shima Minoru Kyōju Taikan Kinen Kokugogaku Ronshū edited by Iwate Kokugo Gakkai Ronshu kankokai, 188–220 (1–33), Morioka: Iwate Kokugo Gakkai Ronshu Kankokai.Search in Google Scholar

Uwano, Zendo. 1996a. “Naze-shi Ashikebu, Arira hōgen no meishi no akusento taikei.” Tōkyō Daigaku Gengogaku Ronshū (15), 3–68.Search in Google Scholar

Uwano, Zendo. 1996b. “Nihongo akusento no saiken.” Gengo Kenkyū 130, 1–42.Search in Google Scholar

Uwano, Zendo. 1998. “Okinoerabujima hōgen no taigen no akusento shiryō.” Journal of Asian and African Studies 27, 131–262.Search in Google Scholar

Uwano, Zendo. 2007. “Okinoerabujima hōgen goi no akusento shiryō.” Ryūkyū no Hōgen 31, 1–38.Search in Google Scholar

Uwano, Zendo. 2010. “Ryūkyū Yonaguni hōgen no akusento shiryō (1).” Ryūkyū no Hōgen (34), 1–30.Search in Google Scholar

Uwano, Zendo. 2013. “Ryūkyū Yonaguni hōgen no akusento shiryō (2).” Ryūkyū no Hōgen (37), 109–142.Search in Google Scholar

Uwano, Zendo. 2014. “Kikaijima hōgen no akusento shiryō.” NINJAL Research Papers 7, 289–310.Search in Google Scholar

Uwano, Zendo. 2017a. “Tokunoshima Asama hōgen no akusento shiryō (3).” NINJAL Research Papers 12, 139–161.Search in Google Scholar

Uwano, Zendo. 2017b. “Chōboin no tanshuku kara kaku ga shōzuru ka: Hattori kasetsu wo megutte.” Journal of Asian and African Studies 94, 345–363.Search in Google Scholar

Vovin, Alexander. 1993. “Long vowels in proto-Japanese.” Journal of East Asian Linguistics 2, 125–134.10.1007/BF01732501Search in Google Scholar

Vovin, Alexander. 2008. “Proto-Japanese beyond the accent system.” In Proto-Japanese: Issues and Prospects edited by Bjarke Frellesvig, John Whitman, 141–156. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.10.1075/cilt.294.11vovSearch in Google Scholar

Vovin, Alexander. 2010. Koreo-Japonica: A Critical Study in the Proposed Language Relationship. Honolulu: University of Hawai’i Press.10.21313/hawaii/9780824832780.001.0001Search in Google Scholar

Received: 2021-06-24
Revised: 2022-04-06
Accepted: 2022-05-26
Published Online: 2022-06-27

© 2022 Yosuke Igarashi, published by De Gruyter

This work is licensed under the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License.

Downloaded on 5.12.2023 from https://www.degruyter.com/document/doi/10.1515/opli-2022-0193/html
Scroll to top button