Skip to content
BY 4.0 license Open Access Published by De Gruyter Open Access December 1, 2022

Mild acute stress prevents the memory impairment induced by long-term isoflurane anesthesia

  • Tiantian Liu , Yutong Dai , Minhui Xu , Ying Chen , Tianjiao Xia EMAIL logo and Xin Zhao EMAIL logo

Abstract

Objectives

Long-term isoflurane anesthesia exposure could result in postoperative cognitive dysfunction (POCD). Preoperative stress is also reported to be a risk factor of POCD. However, it is unknown whether acute stress could impair memory after long-term isoflurane anesthesia.

Methods

In this study, we categorized the mice with acute stress into mild (30 min restraint stress), moderate (60 min restraint stress), and severe (120 min restraint stress) stress groups and then we used Open-Field Test (OFT) to detect whether different scales of acute restraint stress successfully induced acute stress in mice. The memory performance of mice was measured using contextual and cued memory test, and the brain-derived neurotrophic factor protein levels of hippocampus was detected by Western blot.

Results

We verified that mild stress has pro-cognitive effect, but severe stress has amnestic effect. Moreover, we found that mild and moderate other than severe acute stress could partially attenuate the memory impairment induced by long-term isoflurane anesthesia.

Conclusion

Mild and moderate acute stress could partially attenuate the memory impairment induced by long-term isoflurane anesthesia.

1 Introduction

Postoperative cognitive dysfunction (POCD) is characterized by cognitive dysfunction, attention deficit, and executive dysfunction following anesthesia and surgery [1]. Since its first description in 1955 [2], POCD has aroused global concern, due to its association with increased risks of postoperative complications, morbidity, and mortality [3]. According to epidemiological data, up to 26.5% of patients suffer POCD for 1 week after operation, and 9.9% for 3 months [4]. Given the high mortality of POCD [5], there is an urgent need to probe into its pathogenesis. Recently, general anesthesia has been reported to exert detrimental effects on cognition in animal models [6]. Our previous studies have also clarified that long-term isoflurane anesthesia (6 h) could impair spatial memory [7], contextual memory, cued memory, and recognition memory [8].

Accumulating evidence has demonstrated that various etiologies, such as aging, extensive surgery and anesthesia, and low educational level, are involved in the occurrence of POCD [4]. Preoperative stress is also associated with postoperative executive dysfunction and prolonged postoperative recovery [9,10]. Research by Anna et al. shows that more than 40% of cardiac patients experience preoperative stress regarding the disease, surgery, and post-surgery complications [11]. High-level stress, which negatively impacts the central nervous system, is involved in multiple neurological disorders, including depression and posttraumatic stress disorder [12]. By contrast, a lower level of stress seems to be associated with a better cognition in humans and rodents [1315]. To the best of our knowledge, the effect of acute stress on long-term isoflurane anesthesia-induced POCD has never been explored.

In this study, acute stress was induced by restraint stress in the mouse model, which was divided into mild (30 min restraint stress), moderate (60 min restraint stress), and severe (120 min restraint stress) stress groups. Open-Field Test (OFT) verified the successful induction of acute stress by different scales of acute restraint stress (ARS). Fear conditioning test revealed that mild acute stress exerted a pro-cognitive effect, and mild and moderate acute stress partially attenuated the memory impairment induced by long-term isoflurane anesthesia. Acute stress might modulate the anesthesia-related cognitive disorder via targeting the hippocampal brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF).

2 Materials and methods

2.1 Animals

In this study, a total of 184 6–7 weeks male C57/BL-6J mice weighing 20–22 g were used. We tried our best to minimize the number of mice. All animals were kept for at least 3 weeks, with a 12 h light/dark cycle (light on at 8:00 and light off at 20:00) in appropriate room temperature (23 ± 1°C) and free access to food and water.

2.2 ARS

In this study, we categorized acute stress into three different levels: mild, moderate and severe acute stress induced by 30, 60, and 120 min of restraint stress, respectively. The stressed mice were individually restrained in 50 mL conical, transparent polypropylene tubes that were perforated to provide ventilation. Meanwhile, the corresponding unstressed animals were kept undisturbed in their home cages.

2.3 OFT

The OFT was performed as described previously [16]. Briefly, following the restraint stress, each mouse from the stressed and unstressed group was placed in the center of the custom-made Perspex open field with white sides and base (45 cm × 45 cm × 20 cm) for 10 min. An overhead camera recorded the locomotor and exploratory activities of the mice. The time spent in the central zone and the distance traveled in the central zone were automatically recorded with ANYMAZE software. After each trial, the apparatus was cleaned with 75% ethanol solution to eliminate the animal odors and residues.

2.4 General anesthesia

Mice were put in plexiglass chambers and anesthetized under a mixture of 1.3% isoflurane (Tocris, 9A/180370) and 100% oxygen (2–3 L/min). The anesthesia procedure lasted for 6 h, during which an anesthesia gas monitor supervised the inspiratory concentration of isoflurane. We also monitored the respiratory activities, an important vital sign, of mice during the whole procedure.

2.5 Fear conditioning test

The contextual and cued fear conditioning tests were carried out using Panlab Fear Conditioning System (Harvard Apparatus, Spain). Each mouse was trained and tested only once throughout the fear conditioning experiments to avoid familiarization. During the training phase, each mouse was placed in the conditioning chamber and allowed to freely explore the chamber for 3 min followed with a 30 s pulsating tone (80 dB, 2,000 Hz) which ended with a 2 s mild foot shock (0.75 mA). After that, the mice stayed in the chamber for another 1.5 min. Twenty-four hours later (testing phase), the mice were placed in the former chamber for a total of 5 min without sound and shock for the contextual memory test. Two hours later, mice were placed in a new environment, in which the light changed, and each of the chamber walls was installed with a different colored mental board for the cued memory test. Following 3 min free exploration, the same auditory stimulation (80 dB, 2,000 Hz) was given to the mice for 30 s. Animal’s freezing behavior, defined as an utterly immobile posture except for breathing in the test chamber, was continuously recorded using the Packwin 2.0 software. The freezing behavior was then converted to the percentage of freezing time to assess mice’s learning and memory.

2.6 Western blot

After the last behavioral test, all mice were given euthanasia. The hippocampus of mice was isolated and immediately stored at −80°C. Western blot was performed to measure the protein levels of hippocampal BDNF. In brief, the hippocampus tissues were washed to wipe out the circulating blood, dried the surface water, weighed, and homogenized in cold radioimmunoprecipitation assay (RIPA) lysis buffer (Beyotime, China) with proteinase and phosphatase inhibitors (EMD Millipore, USA), and then centrifuged to acquire protein supernatants. The total protein was quantified using the bicinchoninic acid method (Beyotime, China). Equal amounts of protein (40 μg) were loaded into each lane of 10% SDS-PAGE gel and separated by electrophoresis. Proteins were subsequently electrotransferred to a polyvinylidene fluoride membrane (EMD Millipore, USA). The membranes were blocked with 5% (w/v) milk for 1 h, probed by primary antibodies against BDNF (1:1,000, Abcam) and β-actin (1:1,000, Abcam) and HRP-conjugated secondary antibodies diluted in 5% (w/v) milk.

  1. Ethical approval: The research related to animals’ use has been complied with all the relevant national regulations and institutional policies for the care and use of animals. Experiments were performed with the approval of the Laboratory Animal Ethics Committee of Drum Tower Hospital, and all experimental procedures were conducted according to the EU Directive 2010/63/EU for animal experiments. The experiment was approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee, Nanjing University, China (approval No. 20171102).

3 Experimental design

3.1 Experiment 1

In this experiment, a restraint stress model was applied to induce acute stress in mice. Twenty-four mice were randomly divided into 4 groups (6 mice per group): control group (con), mild stress group (mice subjected to 30 min restraint stress), moderate stress group (mice subjected to 60 min restraint stress), and severe stress group (mice subjected to 120 min restraint stress). To assess the efficacy of the restraint model, the mice were put in the open field immediately after the restraint stress, and an overhead camera recorded their locomotor and exploratory behaviors.

3.2 Experiment 2

To verify the effects of different levels of ARS on the fear conditioning memory, 72 mice were randomly divided into the control group, mild stress group, moderate stress group, and severe stress group (18 mice per group). Each group was subjected to different scales of ARS, and then was further split into three subgroups to measure the freezing behavior of mice using the fear conditioning test. One subgroup was measured on the first day after stress exposure (Day 1, n = 6), one subgroup on the third day after stress exposure (Day 3, n = 6), and another subgroup on the seventh day after stress exposure (Day 7, n = 6).

3.3 Experiment 3

To verify the effects of different levels of ARS on the anesthesia-related memory deficits, 88 mice were randomly divided into the control group (Con), anesthesia group (Ane), mild stress + Ane group, moderate stress + Ane group, severe stress + Ane group (16–18 mice per group). Stressed mice were subjected to different scales of restraint stress 30 min before anesthesia. The anesthesia mice were exposed to 6 h isoflurane anesthesia. Similar to Experiment 2, each group was further divided into 3 parts to conduct fear conditioning test on Day 1, Day 3, and Day 7 after stress and anesthesia exposure, respectively. After behavioral tests, all mice were given euthanasia, and the hippocampus was isolated and rapidly preserved at −80°C. We then detected the relative protein levels of BDNF in the hippocampus of mice using western blot.

3.4 Statistical analyses

All data were expressed as the mean value ± standard deviation (SD). Statistical analyses were conducted by one-way ANOVA followed by Bonferroni post hoc tests using SPSS 17.0 software. Significance was defined when P < 0.05.

4 Results

4.1 Effects of different levels of ARS on open-field behavior of mice

To affirm the efficacy of ARS on mice, we measured the open-field behaviors of mice in the OFT. One-way ANOVA analysis revealed the significant effect of ARS on the time spent in the center zone (F (3,23) = 33.744, P = 0.000, Figure 1a), and the post hoc analysis indicated a decrease in the mild stress group (P = 0.000), moderate stress group (P = 0.000), and severe stress group (P = 0.000) as compared to the Con group, respectively. Similarly, ARS significantly affected the distance moved in the center zone (F (3,23) = 11.940, P = 0.000, Figure 1b), with a significant decrease in mild stress group (P = 0.000), moderate stress group (P = 0.003), and severe stress group (P = 0.000) in relation to the Con group. No significant difference was seen among all groups in all traveled distances and average speed throughout the OFT (Figure 1c and d).

Figure 1 
                  Effects of different levels of acute restraint stress on open-field behavior of mice. The time spent in the center of the open field (a). The distance traveled in the center of the open field (b). The mean speed moved in the open field (c). Total distance traveled in the open field (d). All data are presented as mean value ± SD (n = 6/group). **
                     P < 0.01, ***
                     P < 0.001, compared with Con group (One-way ANOVA followed by Bonferroni post hoc test).
Figure 1

Effects of different levels of acute restraint stress on open-field behavior of mice. The time spent in the center of the open field (a). The distance traveled in the center of the open field (b). The mean speed moved in the open field (c). Total distance traveled in the open field (d). All data are presented as mean value ± SD (n = 6/group). ** P < 0.01, *** P < 0.001, compared with Con group (One-way ANOVA followed by Bonferroni post hoc test).

4.2 Effects of different levels of ARS on contextual memory and cued memory of mice

We affirmed the fear conditioning of mice on Day 1, 3, and 7 after different levels of ARS, respectively. In the contextual fear condition test, a significant increase in freezing time was found in the mild stress group ( P = 0.047), and a significant decrease in freezing time was found in the severe stress group (P = 0.001) on Day 3 after anesthesia compared to the Con group (Figure 2a). However, no significant difference in freezing time was found among the 4 groups on Day 1 and Day 7 after anesthesia (P > 0.05, Figure 2a). ARS did not impact the freezing behavior of mice in the cued fear conditioning test (P > 0.05, Figure 2b).

Figure 2 
                  Cognitive performance of mice after induction of different levels of acute restraint stress. Freezing percentage in contextual fear conditioning test (a) and cued fear conditioning test (b). All data are presented as mean value ± SD (n = 6/group). a
                     P < 0.05, compared with the Con group; b
                     P < 0.05, compared with the mild stress group; c
                     P < 0.05, compared with the moderate stress group (One-way ANOVA followed by Bonferroni post hoc test).
Figure 2

Cognitive performance of mice after induction of different levels of acute restraint stress. Freezing percentage in contextual fear conditioning test (a) and cued fear conditioning test (b). All data are presented as mean value ± SD (n = 6/group). a P < 0.05, compared with the Con group; b P < 0.05, compared with the mild stress group; c P < 0.05, compared with the moderate stress group (One-way ANOVA followed by Bonferroni post hoc test).

4.3 Effects of different levels of ARS on contextual memory and cued memory of mice after long-term isoflurane anesthesia

Furthermore, to investigate whether acute stress can modulate the anesthesia-related memory impairment, mice were subjected to different levels of ARS following long-term isoflurane anesthesia, and their memory performance was tested in fear conditioning test on Day 1, Day 3, and Day 7 after anesthesia, respectively (Figure 3). In the contextual fear conditioning test, the Ane group exhibited a significant reduction in freezing time compared with the Con group on Day 1 (P = 0.001), Day 3 (P = 0.002), and Day 7 (P = 0.014) after anesthesia, which was consistent with our previous studies [8,17] (Figure 3a). Moreover, a significant increase in freezing time was presented in mild stress + Ane group (P = 0.000) and moderate stress + Ane group (P = 0.014) compared with the Ane group on Day 1 after anesthesia (Figure 3a). A significant increase in freezing time was also seen between the mild stress + Ane group and the Ane group on Day 7 after anesthesia (P = 0.028, Figure 3a). However, on Day 3 after anesthesia, all levels of restraint stress exposure before anesthesia did not exhibit a significant difference in freezing time compared to those of mice with anesthesia exposure only (P > 0.05, Figure 3a). Prior-anesthesia exposure to ARS did not impact the freezing behavior of aestheticized mice in the cued fear conditioning test (P > 0.05, Figure 3b).

Figure 3 
                  Cognitive performance of mice after induction of different levels of acute restraint stress. Freezing percentage in contextual fear conditioning test (a) and cued fear conditioning test (b). All data are presented as mean value ± SD (n = 4–6/group). a
                     P < 0.05, compared with the Con group; b
                     P < 0.05, compared with the Ane group; c
                     P < 0.05, compared with the mild stress + Ane group; d
                     P < 0.05, compared with the moderate stress + Ane group (One-way ANOVA followed by Bonferroni post hoc test).
Figure 3

Cognitive performance of mice after induction of different levels of acute restraint stress. Freezing percentage in contextual fear conditioning test (a) and cued fear conditioning test (b). All data are presented as mean value ± SD (n = 4–6/group). a P < 0.05, compared with the Con group; b P < 0.05, compared with the Ane group; c P < 0.05, compared with the mild stress + Ane group; d P < 0.05, compared with the moderate stress + Ane group (One-way ANOVA followed by Bonferroni post hoc test).

4.4 Effects of ARS and anesthesia on protein levels of BDNF in the hippocampus of mice

We further determined the BDNF protein levels in the hippocampus of mice using western blot. On Day 1 after anesthesia (Figure 4a), a significant increase in BDNF protein level was found in Ane group (P = 0.002) and mild stress + Ane group (P = 0.023) compared with Con group, and severe stress + Ane group exhibited a significant decrease in BDNF protein level (P = 0.000) compared with Ane group. On Day 3 after anesthesia (Figure 4b), Ane group presented a significant decrease in BDNF protein level compared with Con group (P = 0.012), and a significant increase in BDNF protein level was displayed in mild stress + Ane (P = 0.010) and moderate stress + Ane group (P = 0.001) compared with Ane group. On Day 7 after anesthesia, no significant difference was seen among groups (P > 0.05, Figure 4c).

Figure 4 
                  Effects of acute restraint stress and anesthesia on BDNF protein levels in the hippocampus of mice. Relative protein levels of BDNF in the hippocampus of mice detected on Day 1 (a), Day 3 (b), and Day 7 (c) after anesthesia. All data are presented as mean value ± SD (n = 4/group). *P < 0.05, **P < 0.01, compared with the Con group; #
                     P < 0.05, ##
                     P < 0.01, ###
                     P < 0.001, compared with the Ane group (One-way ANOVA followed by Bonferroni post hoc test).
Figure 4

Effects of acute restraint stress and anesthesia on BDNF protein levels in the hippocampus of mice. Relative protein levels of BDNF in the hippocampus of mice detected on Day 1 (a), Day 3 (b), and Day 7 (c) after anesthesia. All data are presented as mean value ± SD (n = 4/group). *P < 0.05, **P < 0.01, compared with the Con group; # P < 0.05, ## P < 0.01, ### P < 0.001, compared with the Ane group (One-way ANOVA followed by Bonferroni post hoc test).

5 Discussion

Growing evidence suggests that extensive anesthetic exposure brings cytotoxicity to the brain and causes cognitive dysfunction [18,19]. As reported in the previous studies, preoperative stress is associated with increased postoperative complications and prolonged postoperative recovery in clinical practice, especially for surgical patients [9,10]. Although acute stress has been demonstrated to affect learning and memory in both humans and animals [2022], the impact of different scales of acute stress on memory disorders after long-term anesthetic exposure is still poorly understood. In this study, we found that pre-anesthesia exposure to mild acute stress could protect the mice from contextual memory impairments on Day 1 and Day 7 after long-term isoflurane anesthesia and the stress and anesthesia exposure could change the BDNF protein levels in the hippocampus of mice.

The animal model of restraint stress is commonly employed to induce acute or chronic stress [23]. Impaired exploratory activity in OFT of mice after ARS has been verified [24]. Our study also found that mild-, moderate-, and severe-ARS all significantly decreased the distance moved and the time spent in the center of the field among ARS mice compared with non-stress mice. Meanwhile, similar levels of locomotor activity were found when comparing ARS mice and non-stress mice, indicating that the immobility and decreased activity of ARS mice in the central area could not be attributed to any alteration in locomotor activity. Besides, a previous study demonstrated that ARS could induce a cognitive change in the fear conditioning test [25]. Our current study, noticeably, found that ARS elicited cognitive change on Day 3 after stress induction in a time-dependent manner: mild stress exerted a pro-cognitive effect, moderate stress exerted no effect, and severe stress exerted a pro-amnestic effect on the contextual memory of mice. Our findings were consistent with those of the previous studies, revealing that 30 min exposure to ARS could facilitate the acquisition of paired associate learning [26], and longer exposure to ARS could damage recognition memory (90 min restraint stress) [27] and working spatial memory (120 min restraint stress) [28]. And restraint stress of all three lengths could increase the serum corticosterone levels [2830]. Previous literature has pointed out that different concentrations of corticosterone were released in rodents (cortisol in humans) after exposure to different levels of ARS and could exert a reversed U-shaped effect on cognitive function [31]. But in this study, the relationship between the corticosterone levels that could also show the levels of perceived stress after different lengths of ARS and the effect of stress and anesthesia on cognition has not been addressed, which is worth studying in the future.

Extensive anesthetic exposure has been reported to have detrimental effects on brain function, especially memory [19]. For instance, the 4 h sevoflurane inhalation can damage the working memory of mice [32], and repeated exposure to propofol for 6 days can impair the cognition of rats [33]. In line with these observations, our present experiments showed that the 6 h isoflurane anesthesia induced hippocampus-dependent contextual memory deficits from Day 1–7 after anesthesia. Meanwhile, prior exposure to mild and moderate acute stress remarkably rescued the hippocampus-dependent contextual memory deficits on Day 1 after anesthesia, and mild acute stress could also rescue the memory deficits on Day 7 after anesthesia. Hippocampus, a critical brain area involved in learning and memory, is particularly sensitive to stress due to the abundant presence of mineralocorticoid receptors and glucocorticoid receptors (GRs), two classes of corticosteroid receptors that can bind to corticosteroid released after stress exposure [34,35]. A previous study demonstrated that brief restraint stress (30 min) could facilitate long-term potentiation (LTP) in the mouse hippocampus by modulating GRs and ion channels [36], which may explain the anti-amnestic effect of mild acute stress in our study. Besides, animal studies showed that acute stress could alter brain structure and function via multiple interacting mediators, including BDNF [37], tPA [38], CRF [39], and lipocalin-2 [40].

In this study, we focused on the effects of long-term isoflurane anesthesia and different scales of acute stress on BDNF expression in the mouse hippocampus. BDNF, widely expressed in the brain, including the hippocampus plays a critical role in learning and memory [41,42,43]. Previous research has implicated the involvement of BDNF in stress-related mental disorders [44]. In the current study, we found that the hippocampal BDNF expression changed dynamically after the 6 h exposure to isoflurane anesthesia: increasing on Day 1, decreasing on Day 3, and recovering to the control level on Day 7 after anesthesia. Furthermore, the pre-anesthesia exposure to mild acute stress up-regulated BDNF expression on both Day 1 and Day 3 after stress, suggesting a potential mechanism underlying the protective effect of pre-existing mild stress on anesthesia-induced memory deficit.

In conclusion, ARS exposure exerts a time-dependent effect on the contextual memory of mice on Day 3 after stress exposure: mild acute stress improving memory, moderate acute stress having no effect on memory, and severe acute stress damaging memory of mice. Prior exposure to mild and moderate acute stress attenuates the long-term anesthesia-induced memory deficit on Day 1 following anesthesia. Mild acute stress also exerts an anti-amnestic effect on Day 7 following anesthesia. Besides, BDNF in the hippocampus may play a critical role in this process. Our study still has some limitations. For instance, only fear conditioning test was conducted in the study, which is relatively unitary cognitive assessment and could not exclude the affective effect, another hippocampus-based learning test such as T-maze and water maze tests could also be conducted. Meanwhile, given previous research works showed conflicting results related to the effects of stress on female mice in fear conditioning test [45,46], we just used male mice in this study. Sex difference in the effect of acute stress and anesthesia on cognition could be investigated in the future work using more behavior tests.


# Tiantian Liu and Yutong Dai contributed equally to this work and should be considered as co-first authors.


Acknowledgments

We thank Professor Zhongzhou Yang and Lizhi Xu for technical assistance.

  1. Funding information: This research was supported by the National Natural Science Foundation (81801380, 81701371).

  2. Author contributions: Study concept and design: Tiantian Liu and Yutong Dai; data acquisition: Tiantian Liu, Yutong Dai, and Ying Chen; analysis and interpretation: Tiantian Liu, Yutong Dai, and Minhui Xu; critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content: Tianjiao Xia and Xin Zhao.

  3. Conflict of interest: Authors state no conflict of interest.

  4. Data availability statement: The datasets generated during and/or analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

References

[1] Newman MF, Kirchner JL, Phillips-Bute B, Gaver V, Grocott H, Jones RH, et al. Longitudinal assessment of neurocognitive function after coronary-artery bypass surgery. Cardiopulm Phys Ther J. 2001;12(2):57. 10.1097/01823246-200112020-00007.Search in Google Scholar

[2] Bedford PD. Adverse cerebral effects of anaesthesia on old people. Lancet. Aug. 1955;269(6884):259–63. 10.1016/s0140-6736(55)92689-1.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

[3] Evered L, Silbert B, Knopman DS, Scott DA, DeKosky ST, Rasmussen LS, et al. Recommendations for the nomenclature of cognitive change associated with anaesthesia and surgery-20181. J Alzheimers Dis. 2018;66(1):1–10. 10.3233/JAD-189004.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

[4] Moller JT, Cluitmans P, Rasmussen LS, Houx P, Rasmussen H, Canet J, et al. Long-term postoperative cognitive dysfunction in the elderly: ISPOCD1 study. Lancet. 1998;351(9106):857–61. 10.1016/S0140-6736(97)07382-0.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

[5] Steinmetz J, Bang Christensen K, Lund T, Lohse N, Rasmussen LS. Long-term consequences of postoperative cognitive dysfunction. Anesthesiology. 2009;110(3):548–55. 10.1097/ALN.0b013e318195b569.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

[6] Wang Z, Meng S, Cao L, Chen Y, Zuo Z, Peng S. Critical role of NLRP3-caspase-1 pathway in age-dependent isoflurane-induced microglial inflammatory response and cognitive impairment. J Neuroinflammation. 2018;15(1):1–12. 10.1186/s12974-018-1137-1.Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

[7] Song J, Chu S, Cui Y, Qian Y, Li X, Xu F, et al. Circadian rhythm resynchronization improved isoflurane-induced cognitive dysfunction in aged mice. Exp Neurol. 2018;306(no. April):45–54. 10.1016/j.expneurol.2018.04.009.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

[8] Song Y, Li X, Gong X, Zhao X, Ma Z, Xia T, et al. Green tea polyphenols improve isoflurane-induced cognitive impairment via modulating oxidative stress. J Nutr Biochem. 2019;73:108213. 10.1016/j.jnutbio.2019.07.004.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

[9] Du J, Plas M, Absalom AR, Leeuwen BL, Bock GH. The association of preoperative anxiety and depression with neurocognitive disorder following oncological surgery. J Surg Oncol. Mar. 2020;121(4):676–87. 10.1002/jso.25836.Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

[10] Stamenkovic DM, Rancic NK, Latas MB, Neskovic V, Rondovic GM, Wu JD, et al. Preoperative anxiety and implications on postoperative recovery: What can we do to change our history. Minerva Anestesiol. Nov. 2018;84(11):1307–17. 10.23736/S0375-9393.18.12520-X.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

[11] Rosiek A, Kornatowski T, Rosiek-Kryszewska A, Leksowski Ł, Leksowski K. Evaluation of stress intensity and anxiety level in preoperative period of cardiac patients. Biomed Res Int. 2016;2016:1–8. 10.1155/2016/1248396.Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

[12] Bangasser DA, Valentino RJ. Sex differences in stress-related psychiatric disorders: Neurobiological perspectives. Front Neuroendocrinol. Aug. 2014;35(3):303–19. 10.1016/j.yfrne.2014.03.008.Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

[13] Gómez-Gallego M, Gómez-García J. Stress and verbal memory in patients with Alzheimer’s disease: Different role of cortisol and anxiety. Aging Ment Health. Nov. 2019;23(11):1496–502. 10.1080/13607863.2018.1506741.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

[14] Bierman EJM, Comijs HC, Rijmen F, Jonker C, Beekman ATF. Anxiety symptoms and cognitive performance in later life: Results from the longitudinal aging study Amsterdam. Aging Ment Heal. 2008;12(4):517–23. 10.1080/13607860802224276.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

[15] Brivio P, Sbrini G, Riva MA, Calabrese F. Acute stress induces cognitive improvement in the novel object recognition task by transiently modulating BDNF in the prefrontal cortex of male rats. Cell Mol Neurobiol. 2020;40(0123456789):1037–47. 10.1007/s10571-020-00793-7.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

[16] Desbonnet L, O'tuathaigh CM, O'Leary C, Cox R, Tighe O, Petit EI, et al. Acute stress in adolescence vs early adulthood following selective deletion of dysbindin-1A: Effects on anxiety, cognition and other schizophrenia-related phenotypes. J Psychopharmacol. Dec. 2019;33(12):1610–9. 10.1177/0269881119875465.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

[17] Liu H, Gong XD, Zhao X, Qian Y, Gu XP, Xia TJ. GLYX-13 pretreatment ameliorates long-term isoflurane exposure-induced cognitive impairment in mice. Neural Regen Res. 2020;15(1):128–35. 10.4103/1673-5374.264466.Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

[18] Eckenhoff RG, Johansson JS, Wei H, Carnini A, Kang B, Wei W, et al. Inhaled anesthetic enhancement of amyloid-β oligomerization and cytotoxicity. Anesthesiology. Sep. 2004;101(3):703–9. 10.1097/00000542-200409000-00019.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

[19] Wu L, Zhao H, Weng H, Ma D. Lasting effects of general anesthetics on the brain in the young and elderly: ‘mixed picture’ of neurotoxicity, neuroprotection and cognitive impairment. J Anesth. 2019;33(2):321–35. 10.1007/s00540-019-02623-7.Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

[20] Shields GS, Sazma MA, McCullough AM, Yonelinas AP. The effects of acute stress on episodic memory: A meta-analysis and integrative review. Psychol Bull. Jun. 2017;143(6):636–75. 10.1037/bul0000100.Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

[21] Wolf OT, Atsak P, de Quervain DJ, Roozendaal B, Wingenfeld K. Stress and Memory: A selective review on recent developments in the understanding of stress hormone effects on memory and their clinical relevance. J Neuroendocrinol. Aug. 2016;28:8. 10.1111/jne.12353.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

[22] Ness D, Calabrese P. Stress effects on multiple memory system interactions. Neural Plast. 2016;2016:1–20. 10.1155/2016/4932128.Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

[23] Manchanda RK, Jaggi AS, Singh N. Ameliorative potential of sodium cromoglycate and diethyldithiocarbamic acid in restraint stress-induced behavioral alterations in rats. Pharmacol Rep. 2011;63(1):54–63. 10.1016/S1734-1140(11)70398-X.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

[24] Amin SN, Hassan SS, Khashaba AS, Youakim MF, Latif N, Rashed LA, et al. Hippocampal and cerebellar changes in acute restraint stress and the impact of pretreatment with ceftriaxone. Brain Sci. Mar. 2020;10(4):193. 10.3390/brainsci10040193.Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

[25] Cordero MI, Venero C, Kruyt ND, Sandi C. Prior exposure to a single stress session facilitates subsequent contextual fear conditioning in rats. Horm Behav. Nov. 2003;44(4):338–45. 10.1016/S0018-506X(03)00160-0.Search in Google Scholar

[26] Roebuck AJ, Liu MC, Lins BR, Scott GA, Howland JG. Acute stress, but not corticosterone, facilitates acquisition of paired associates learning in rats using touchscreen-equipped operant conditioning chambers. Behav Brain Res. Aug. 2018;348(April):139–49. 10.1016/j.bbr.2018.04.027.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

[27] Guercio GD, Bevictori L, Vargas-Lopes C, Madeira C, Oliveira A, Carvalho VF, et al. D-serine prevents cognitive deficits induced by acute stress. Neuropharmacology. Nov. 2014;86:1–8. 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2014.06.021.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

[28] Amin SN, El-Aidi AA, Ali MM, Attia YM, Rashed LA. Modification of hippocampal markers of synaptic plasticity by memantine in animal models of acute and repeated restraint stress: Implications for memory and behavior. NeuroMolecular Med. 2015;17(2):121–36. 10.1007/s12017-015-8343-0.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

[29] Schalla MA, Goebel-Stengel M, Friedrich T, Kühne SG, Kobelt P, Rose M, et al. Restraint stress affects circulating NUCB2/nesfatin-1 and phoenixin levels in male rats. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2020;122(July):104906. 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2020.104906.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

[30] Roper JA, Craighead M, O’Carroll AM, Lolait SJ. Attenuated stress response to acute restraint and forced swimming stress in arginine vasopressin 1b receptor subtype (Avpr1b) receptor knockout mice and wild-type mice treated with a novel Avpr1b receptor antagonist. J Neuroendocrinol. 2010;22(11):1173–80. 10.1111/j.1365-2826.2010.02070.x.Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

[31] Sandi C, Pinelo-Nava MT. Stress and memory: Behavioral effects and neurobiological mechanisms. Neural Plast. 2007;2007:78970. 10.1155/2007/78970.Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

[32] Shan L, Ma D, Zhang C, Xiong W, Zhang Y. miRNAs may regulate GABAergic transmission associated genes in aged rats with anesthetics-induced recognition and working memory dysfunction. Brain Res. 2017;1670:191–200. 10.1016/j.brainres.2017.06.027.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

[33] Liu PF, Gao T, Li TZ, Yang YT, Xu YX, Xu ZP, et al. Repeated propofol exposure-induced neuronal damage and cognitive impairment in aged rats by activation of NF-κB pathway and NLRP3 inflammasome. Neurosci Lett. 2021;740(September 2020):135461. 10.1016/j.neulet.2020.135461.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

[34] Kim JJ, Diamond DM. The stressed hippocampus, synaptic plasticity and lost memories. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2002;3(6):453–62. 10.1038/nrn849.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

[35] Kim JJ, Song EY, Kosten TA. Stress effects in the hippocampus: Synaptic plasticity and memory. Stress. 2006;9(1):1–11. 10.1080/10253890600678004.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

[36] Wang M, Ramasamy VS, Samidurai M, Jo J. Acute restraint stress reverses impaired LTP in the hippocampal CA1 region in mouse models of Alzheimer’s disease. Sci Rep. 2019;9(1):1–9. 10.1038/s41598-019-47452-6.Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

[37] McEwen BS, Bowles NP, Gray JD, Hill MN, Hunter RG, Karatsoreos IN, et al. Mechanisms of stress in the brain. Nat Neurosci. Oct. 2015;18(10):1353–63. 10.1038/nn.4086.Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

[38] Pawlak R, Rao BSS, Melchor JP, Chattarji S, McEwen B, Strickland S. Tissue plasminogen activator and plasminogen mediate stress-induced decline of neuronal and cognitive functions in the mouse hippocampus. Proc Natl Acad Sci. Dec. 2005;102(50):18201–6. 10.1073/pnas.0509232102.Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

[39] Chen Y, Fenoglio KA, Dubé CM, Grigoriadis DE, Baram TZ. Cellular and molecular mechanisms of hippocampal activation by acute stress are age-dependent. Mol Psychiatry. Nov. 2006;11(11):992–1002. 10.1038/sj.mp.4001863.Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

[40] Mucha M, Skrzypiec AE, Schiavon E, Attwood BK, Kucerova E, Pawlak R. Lipocalin-2 controls neuronal excitability and anxiety by regulating dendritic spine formation and maturation. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2011;108(45):18436–41. 10.1073/pnas.1107936108.Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

[41] Kowiański P, Lietzau G, Czuba E, Waśkow M, Steliga A, Moryś J. BDNF: A key factor with multipotent impact on brain signaling and synaptic plasticity. Cell Mol Neurobiol. 2018;38(3):579–93. 10.1007/s10571-017-0510-4.Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

[42] Lu B, Gottschalk W. Modulation of hippocampal synaptic transmission and plasticity by neurotrophins. Prog Brain Res. 2000;128:231–41. 10.1016/S0079-6123(00)28020-5.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

[43] Egan MF, Kojima M, Callicott JH, Goldberg TE, Kolachana BS, Bertolino A, et al. The BDNF val66met polymorphism affects activity-dependent secretion of BDNF and human memory and hippocampal function. Cell. 2003;112(2):257–69. 10.1016/S0092-8674(03)00035-7.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

[44] Notaras M, van den Buuse M. Neurobiology of BDNF in fear memory, sensitivity to stress, and stress-related disorders. Mol Psychiatry. 2020;25:2251–74. 10.1038/s41380-019-0639-2.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

[45] Gutknecht L, Popp S, Waider J, Sommerlandt FM, Göppner C, Post A, et al. Interaction of brain 5-HT synthesis deficiency, chronic stress and sex differentially impact emotional behavior in Tph2 knockout mice. Psychopharmacology. 2015;232(14):2429–41. 10.1007/s00213-015-3879-0.Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

[46] Mahmoud ME, Rehan IF, KEl-DawyAhmed, Abdelrahman A, Mohammadi S, Abou-Elnaga AF, et al. Identification of serum N-glycoproteins as a biological correlate underlying chronic stress response in mice. Mol Biol Rep. 2019;0(0):0–2748. 10.1007/s11033-019-04717-7.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

Received: 2022-07-10
Revised: 2022-10-26
Accepted: 2022-11-02
Published Online: 2022-12-01

© 2022 the author(s), published by De Gruyter

This work is licensed under the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License.

Downloaded on 30.9.2023 from https://www.degruyter.com/document/doi/10.1515/tnsci-2022-0261/html
Scroll to top button