Jump to ContentJump to Main Navigation
Show Summary Details
More options …

Applied Linguistics Review

Editor-in-Chief: Wei, Li

4 Issues per year


IMPACT FACTOR 2016: 0.351

Online
ISSN
1868-6311
See all formats and pricing
More options …

Bilinguals and knowledge of language: a commentary to “Language Acquisition and Linguistic Theory”

Itziar Laka
  • Corresponding author
  • Department of Linguistics and Basque Studies, University of the Basque Country UPV/EHU, Justo Vélez de Elorriaga 1, 01006 Vitoria-Gasteiz, Araba, Spain
  • Email
  • Other articles by this author:
  • De Gruyter OnlineGoogle Scholar
Published Online: 2017-11-08 | DOI: https://doi.org/10.1515/applirev-2017-0096

Abstract

How infant and adult humans learn languages and what this tells us about the various aspects of the language system is currently a central topic in linguistic research, one that more and more includes bilingualism and language attrition in its embrace. Despite the centrality of the problem of language acquisition for Generative Grammar, it is fair to say that other modes of language learning are now entering the arena of discussion, having shown their potential for discovery in Cognitive Science at large. Some aspects of grammatical knowledge that are hard to acquire and yield differences between native and non-native speakers reveal that language learning is indeed a special type of learning, which in turn reveals that knowing more about these entails knowing more about language.

Keywords: language acquisition; Bilingualism; Generative Grammar

References

  • Baker, M. 2001. The atoms of language. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar

  • Baker, M. 2003. Linguistic differences and language design. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 7(8). 349–353.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bel, A., E. García-Alcaraz & E. Rosado. 2016. Reference comprehension and production in bilingual Spanish: The view from null subject languages. In Anahí Alba de la Fuente, Elena Valenzuela & Cristina Martínez-Sanz (eds.), Language acquisition beyond parameters, Studies in honour of Juana M. Liceras, 37–70. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar

  • Bosch, L. & N. Sebastián-Gallés. 2001. Evidence of early language discrimination abilities in infants from Bilingual environments. Infancy 2(1). 29–49.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Chomsky, N. 1959. Verbal behavior. By B. F. Skinner. Language 35. 26–58.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Chomsky, N. 1981. Lectures on Government and binding. Dordrecht: Foris.Google Scholar

  • Chomsky, N. 1986. Knowledge of language: Its nature, origins and use. New York: Praeger.Google Scholar

  • Chomsky, N. 1995. The minimalist program. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar

  • Costa, A. & M. Santesteban. 2004. Lexical access in bilingual speech production: Evidence from language switching in highly proficient bilinguals and L2 learners. Journal of Memory and Language 50(4). 491–511.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Domínguez, L. 2013. Understanding interfaces: Second language acquisition and native language attrition of Spanish subject realization and word order variation. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar

  • Evans, N. & S. C. Levinson. 2009. The myth of language universals: Language diversity and its importance for cognitive science. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 32. 429–492.Web of ScienceCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gervain, J., F. Macagno, S. Cogoi, M. Peña & J. Mehler. 2008. The neonate brain detects speech structure. PNAS 105(37). 14222–14227.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Jakobson, R. 1941. Kindersprache, aphasie und allgemeine lautgesetze. English version (1968) Child Language, Aphasia and Phonological Universals. New York: Mouton Publishers.Google Scholar

  • Kayne, R. 1994. The antisymmetry of syntax. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar

  • Montrul, S. 2011. Interfaces and incomplete acquisition. Lingua 121(4). 591–604.Web of ScienceCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Montrul, S. & C. Rodríguez Louro. 2006. Beyond the syntax of the null subject parameter: A look at the discourse-pragmatic distribution of null and overt subjects by L2 learners of Spanish. In Vincent Torrens & Linda Escobar (eds.), The acquisition of syntax in romance languages, 401–418. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar

  • Roberts, I. & A. Roussou. 2003. Syntactic change: A minimalist approach to grammaticalization. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar

  • Rooryck, J., N.V. Smith, A. Liptak & D. Blakemore. 2010. The Myth of language universals. Lingua 120(12). 2651–27.Web of ScienceGoogle Scholar

  • Saffran, J., R. Aslin & E. Newport. 1996. Statistical learning by 8-month old infants. Science 274. 1926–1928.CrossrefWeb of ScienceGoogle Scholar

  • Schütze, C. & J. Sprouse. 2014. Judgment Data. In Devyani Sharma & Rob Podesva (eds.), Research methods in linguistics, 27–50. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar

  • Sorace, A. 2003. Near-nativeness. In M. Long & C. Doughty (eds.), Handbook of second language acquisition, 130–152. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar

  • Sorace, A. 2011. Pinning down the concept of “interface” in bilingualism. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism 1(1). 1–33.Web of ScienceCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Sorace, A. & F. Filiaci. 2006. Anaphora resolution in near-native speakers of Italian. Second Language Research 22(3). 339–368.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Werker, J.F. & R.C. Tees. 1984. Cross-language speech perception: Evidence for perceptual reorganization during the first year of life. Infant Behavior Development 7. 49–63.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • White, Lydia. 1989. Universal grammar and second language acquisition. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar

  • White, Lydia. 2003. Second language acquisition and Universal Grammar. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar

  • Yang, C.L. 2002. Knowledge and learning in natural language. Ney York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar

  • Zawiszewski, A., E. Gutiérrez, B. Fernández & I. Laka. 2011. Language distance and non-native syntactic processing: Evidence from event-related potentials. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 14(03). 400–411.CrossrefWeb of ScienceGoogle Scholar

About the article

Published Online: 2017-11-08


This work was supported by the European Union’s Seventh Framework Programme for research, technological development and demonstration [613465 (AThEME)]; Spanish Ministerio de Economía y Competitividad [FFI2015-64183-P]; Basque Government [IT665-13].


Citation Information: Applied Linguistics Review, ISSN (Online) 1868-6311, ISSN (Print) 1868-6303, DOI: https://doi.org/10.1515/applirev-2017-0096.

Export Citation

© 2017 Walter de Gruyter GmbH, Berlin/Boston. Copyright Clearance Center

Comments (0)

Please log in or register to comment.
Log in