Jump to ContentJump to Main Navigation
Show Summary Details
More options …

Corpus Linguistics and Linguistic Theory

Founded by Gries, Stefan Th. / Stefanowitsch, Anatol

Ed. by Wulff, Stefanie


IMPACT FACTOR 2017: 1.200
5-year IMPACT FACTOR: 1.386

CiteScore 2017: 0.80

SCImago Journal Rank (SJR) 2017: 0.288
Source Normalized Impact per Paper (SNIP) 2017: 0.930

Online
ISSN
1613-7035
See all formats and pricing
More options …

Entrenchment and persistence in language change: the Spanish past subjunctive

Malte Rosemeyer
  • Corresponding author
  • Department of Linguistics, Katholieke Universiteit Leuven, Leuven, Belgium
  • Romanisches Seminar, Albert-Ludwigs-Universitaet Freiburg, Freiburg im Breisgau, Baden-Württemberg, Germany
  • Email
  • Other articles by this author:
  • De Gruyter OnlineGoogle Scholar
/ Scott A. Schwenter
Published Online: 2017-02-14 | DOI: https://doi.org/10.1515/cllt-2016-0047

Abstract

In this paper, we demonstrate that, like frequency, morphosyntactic persistence can have a conserving effect on language change. To substantiate this claim, we analyze the alternation between the Spanish past subjunctive forms ending in –ra and –se (as in comiera and comiese ‘had eaten’). Due to the ongoing replacement of –se by –ra, persistence and frequency are the best predictors of the alternation in our data. First, the persistence effect of a prior –se is significantly greater than the persistence effect of a prior –ra. Second, although –se is basically restricted to third person singular morphology in contexts without persistence, when primed by –se this restriction is drastically reduced. Our results also shed light on the relationship between frequency and persistence in language change. Although both result in conservation, the conserving effect of frequency causes irregularity such as the paradigmatic atrophy of Spanish –se forms. In contrast, persistence can temporarily re-establish paradigmatic regularity and consequently strengthen the cognitive representation of obsolescing constructions. However, this resuscitating effect of persistence appears to be restricted to low-frequency –se forms; because they are generally more entrenched, the activation of high-frequency –se forms relies less on persistence effects.

Keywords: language change; persistence; priming; frequency; usage-based linguistics; past subjunctive; Spanish

References

  • Asratián, Arucia. 2007. Variación –ra/–se en el español hablado en Caracas. Boletín de lingüística 19(5). 5–41.Google Scholar

  • Baayen, Harald. 2008. Analyzing linguistic data. A practical introduction to statistics using R. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar

  • Bock, Kathrin J. & Zenzi M. Griffin. 2000. The persistence of structural priming: Transient activation or implicit learning? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 129(2). 177–192.Google Scholar

  • Bock, Kathryn J. 1986. Syntactic persistence in language production. Cognitive Psychology 18. 355–387.Google Scholar

  • Branigan, Holly, Martin Pickering & Alexandra Cleland. 1999. Syntactic priming in written production: Evidence for rapid decay. Psychological Bulletin and Review 6. 635–640.Google Scholar

  • Breiman, Leo. 2001. Random forests. Machine Learning 45. 5–32.Google Scholar

  • Breiman, Leo, Jerome Friedman, Charles J. Stone & Richard A. Olshen. 1984. Classification and regression trees. New York: Chapman and Hall.Google Scholar

  • Bybee, Joan L. 2003. Mechanisms of change in grammaticization: The role of frequency. In Richard Janda & Brian Joseph (eds.), The handbook of historical linguistics, 624–647. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar

  • Bybee, Joan L. 2006. From usage to grammar: The mind’s response to repetition. Language 82(4). 711–733.Google Scholar

  • Bybee, Joan L. 2010. Language, usage, and cognition. Cambridge, NY: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar

  • Bybee, Joan L. & Rena Torres Cacoullos. 2009. The role of prefabs in grammaticization: How the particular and the general interact in language change. In Roberta Corrigan, Edith A. Moravcsik, Hamid Ouali & Kathleen M. Wheatley (eds.), Formulaic language, volume I: Distribution and historical change, 187–217. Amsterdam & Philadelphia: Benjamins.Google Scholar

  • Croft, William. 2001. Radical construction grammar. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar

  • Davies, Mark. 2002. Corpus del español (100 million words, 1200s-1900s). http://www.corpusdelespanol.org (accessed 22 December 2015).

  • Day, Meagan. 2011. Variation in the use of the –ra and –se forms of the imperfect subjunctive in Modern Spoken Peninsular Spanish. NWAV 40, Georgetown University.

  • DeMello, George. 1993. –ra vs. –se subjunctive: A new look at an old topic. Hispania 76(2). 235–243.Google Scholar

  • Diessel, Holger. 2011. Review article of ‘Language, usage and cognition’ by Joan Bybee. Language 87. 830–844.Google Scholar

  • Eckardt, Regine. 2008. Concept priming in language change. Theoretical Linguistics 34(2). 123–133.Google Scholar

  • Erker, Daniel & Gregory R. Guy. 2012. The role of lexical frequency in syntactic variability: Variable subject personal pronoun expression in Spanish. Language 88(3). 526–557.Google Scholar

  • Ferreira, Victor S. 2003. The persistence of optional complementizer mention: Why saying a “that” is not saying “that” at all. Journal of Memory and Language 48. 379–398.Google Scholar

  • Fillmore, Charles J., Paul Kay & Mary C. O’Connor. 1988. Regularity and idiomaticity in grammatical constructions. Language 64. 501–538.Google Scholar

  • Forster, Kenneth I. & Chris Davis. 1984. Repetition priming and frequency attenuation in lexical access. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 10. 680–698.Google Scholar

  • Goldberg, Adele E. 1995. Constructions: A construction grammar approach to argument structure. Chicago, London: The University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar

  • Goldberg, Adele E. 2006. Constructions at work: The nature of generalization in language. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar

  • Goldberg, Adele E. 2013. Constructionist approaches. In Graeme Trousdale & Thomas Hoffman (eds.), The Oxford handbook of construction grammar. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar

  • Gries, Stefan Th. 2005. Syntactic priming: A corpus-based approach. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 34(4). 365–399.Google Scholar

  • Gries, Stefan Th. & Stefanie Wulff. 2009. Psycholinguistic and corpus-linguistic evidence for L2 constructions. Annual Review of Cognitive Linguistics 7. 163–186.Google Scholar

  • Guzmán Naranjo, Matías. To appear. The se-ra alternation in Spanish subjunctive. Corpus Linguistics and Linguistic Theory. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1515/cllt-2015-0017, February 2016.Crossref

  • Hartsuiker, Robert J. & Herman J. Kolk. 1998. Syntactic persistence in Dutch. Language and Speech 41(2). 143–184.Google Scholar

  • Haverkate, Henk. 2002. The syntax, semantics and pragmatics of Spanish mood. Amsterdam & Philadelphia: John Benjamins.Google Scholar

  • Hilpert, Martin. 2014. Relating language change to language processing: A second look at asymmetric priming. Paper presented at ICEHL18, Leuven.Google Scholar

  • Hothorn, Torsten, Kurt Hornik & Achim Zeileis. 2006. Unbiased recursive partioning: A conditional inference framework. Journal of Computational and Graphical Statistics 15(3). 651–674.Google Scholar

  • Jaeger, T. Florian & Neal Snider. 2008. Implicit learning and syntactic persistence: Surprisal and cumulativity. In Bradley C. Love, Ken McRae & Vladimir M. Sloutsky (eds.), Proceedings of the 30th annual conference of the cognitive science society, 1061–1066. Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society.Google Scholar

  • Jäger, Gerhard & Anette Rosenbach. 2008. Priming and unidirectional language change. Theoretical Linguistics 34(2). 85–113.Google Scholar

  • Kempas, Ilpo. 2011. Sobre la variación en el marco de la libre elección entre cantara y cantase en el español peninsular. Moenia 17. 243–264.Google Scholar

  • Langacker, Ronald W. 1987. Foundations of cognitive grammar. Theoretical prerequisites. Stanford: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar

  • Leech, Geoffrey, Marianne Hundt, Christian Mair & Nicholas Smith. 2009. Change in contemporary English. A grammatical study. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar

  • Lemon, Francis. 1925. The relative frequency of the subjunctive forms in –se and –ra. Hispania 8. 300–302.Google Scholar

  • Lope Blanch, Juan M. (ed.). 1977. Estudios sobre el español hablado en las principales ciudades de América. México: Universidad Nacional Autónoma de México.Google Scholar

  • Lunn, Patricia V. 1995. The evaluative function of the Spanish subjunctive. In Joan Bybee & Suzanne Fleischman (eds.), Modality in grammar and discourse, 429–449. Amsterdam & Philadelphia: John Benjamins.Google Scholar

  • Pickering, Martin J. & Holly P. Branigan. 1998. The representation of verbs: Evidence from syntactic priming in language production. Journal of Memory and Language 39(4). 633–651.Google Scholar

  • R Development Core Team. 2015. R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria.http://www.R-project.org (accessed 26 December 2015).

  • Real Academia Española. 2010. Nueva gramática de la lengua española. Manual. Madrid: Escasa Libros.Google Scholar

  • Rosemeyer, Malte. 2014. Auxiliary selection in Spanish. Gradience, gradualness, and conservation. Amsterdam & Philadelphia: John Benjamins.Google Scholar

  • Rosemeyer, Malte. 2015. How usage rescues the system: Persistence as conservation. In Aria Adli, Marco García García & Göz Kaufmann (eds.), Variation in language: System- and usage-based approaches, 289–311. Berlin, NY: De Gruyter.Google Scholar

  • Schwarz, Christian. 2016. Recency as a factor of phonological variation. In Heike Behrens & Stefan Pfänder (eds.), Experience counts: Frequency effects in language, 91–109. Berlin, NY: De Gruyter.Google Scholar

  • Schwenter, Scott A. 2013. Strength of priming and the maintenance of variation in the Spanish past subjunctive. Paper presented at NWAV 2013, Pittsburgh.Google Scholar

  • Strobl, Carolin, James Malley & Gerhard Tutz. 2009. An introduction to recursive partitioning: Rationale, application, and characteristics of classification and regression trees, bagging, and random forests. Psychological Methods 14(4). 323–348.Google Scholar

  • Sussman Goldberg, Barbara. 1995. The –ra and –se opposition in Spanish. In Ellen Contini-Morava & Barbara Sussman Goldberg (eds.), Meaning as explanation. Advances in linguistic sign theory, 381–404. Berlin, NY: De Gruyter.Google Scholar

  • Szmrecsanyi, Benedikt. 2005. Language users as creatures of habit: A corpus-based analysis of persistence in spoken English. Corpus Linguistics and Linguistic Theory 1(1). 113–150.Google Scholar

  • Szmrecsanyi, Benedikt. 2006. Morphosyntactic persistence in spoken English. A corpus study at the intersection of variationist sociolinguistics, psycholinguistics, and discourse analysis. Berlin, NY: De Gruyter.Google Scholar

  • Tagliamonte, Sali & Harald Baayen. 2012. Models, forests and trees of York English: Was/were variation as a case study for statistical practice. Language Variation and Change 24(2). 135–178.Google Scholar

  • Tamminga, Meredith. 2014. Persistence in the production of linguistic variation. Philadelphia, PA: University of Pennsylvania dissertation.Google Scholar

  • Torres Cacoullos, Rena. 2015. Gradual loss of analyzability: Diachronic priming effects. In Aria Adli, Marco García García & Göz Kaufmann (eds.), Variation in language: System- and usage-based approaches, 265–287. Berlin, NY: De Gruyter.Google Scholar

  • Valeš, Miroslav. 2006. El prestigio desigual de las formas del imperfecto de subjunctivo cantara/cantase. In Juan A. Moya Corral & Marcin Sosinski (eds.), Lexicografía y enseñanza de la lengua española. Actas de las XI Jornadas sobre la enseñanza de la lengua española, 303–311. Granada: Universidad de Granada.Google Scholar

  • Wright, Leavitt O. 1926. The indicative forms in –ra in Spanish America. Hispania 9. 288–293.Google Scholar

About the article

Published Online: 2017-02-14


DFG Graduate School GRK 1624/1 “Frequency effects”; Research Fund Flanders (FWO), award K1G3316N; 2015–16 OSU Arts and Humanities Larger Grant Award for the project “Persistence and the Maintenance of Linguistic Variation.”


Citation Information: Corpus Linguistics and Linguistic Theory, ISSN (Online) 1613-7035, ISSN (Print) 1613-7027, DOI: https://doi.org/10.1515/cllt-2016-0047.

Export Citation

©2017 by De Gruyter Mouton.Get Permission

Citing Articles

Here you can find all Crossref-listed publications in which this article is cited. If you would like to receive automatic email messages as soon as this article is cited in other publications, simply activate the “Citation Alert” on the top of this page.

[1]
Roberta Amato, Lucas Lacasa, Albert Díaz-Guilera, and Andrea Baronchelli
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 2018, Page 201721059

Comments (0)

Please log in or register to comment.
Log in