Jump to ContentJump to Main Navigation
Show Summary Details
More options …

Cognitive Linguistics

Editor-in-Chief: Divjak, Dagmar


IMPACT FACTOR 2017: 1.902
5-year IMPACT FACTOR: 2.297

CiteScore 2018: 2.09

SCImago Journal Rank (SJR) 2018: 1.075
Source Normalized Impact per Paper (SNIP) 2018: 2.063

Online
ISSN
1613-3641
See all formats and pricing
More options …
Volume 23, Issue 2

Issues

The effect of frequency and phonological neighbourhood density on the acquisition of past tense verbs by Finnish children

Minna Kirjavainen, / Alexandre Nikolaev, / Evan Kidd,
Published Online: 2012-04-17 | DOI: https://doi.org/10.1515/cog-2012-0009

Abstract

The acquisition of the past tense has received substantial attention in the psycholinguistics literature, yet most studies report data from English or closely related Indo-European languages. We report on a past tense elicitation study on 136 4–6-year-old children that were acquiring a highly inflected Finno-Ugric (Uralic) language—Finnish. The children were tested on real and novel verbs (N = 120) exhibiting (1) productive, (2) semi-productive, or (3) non-productive inflectional processes manipulated for frequency and phonological neighbourhood density (PND). We found that Finnish children are sensitive to lemma/base frequency and PND when processing inflected words, suggesting that even though children were using suffixation processes, they were also paying attention to the item level properties of the past tense verbs. This paper contributes to the growing body of research suggesting a single analogical/associative mechanism is sufficient in processing both productive (i.e., regular-like) and non-productive (i.e., irregular-like) words. We argue that seemingly rule-like elements in inflectional morphology are an emergent property of the lexicon.

Keywords:: Language acquisition; Finnish; Inflectional morphology; Past tense

About the article

Received: 2010-07-16

Accepted: 2011-11-20

Published Online: 2012-04-17

Published in Print: 2012-05-25


Citation Information: , Volume 23, Issue 2, Pages 273–315, ISSN (Online) 1613-3641, ISSN (Print) 0936-5907, DOI: https://doi.org/10.1515/cog-2012-0009.

Export Citation

©[2012] by Walter de Gruyter Berlin Boston.Get Permission

Citing Articles

Here you can find all Crossref-listed publications in which this article is cited. If you would like to receive automatic email messages as soon as this article is cited in other publications, simply activate the “Citation Alert” on the top of this page.

[3]
Sonia Granlund, Joanna Kolak, Virve Vihman, Felix Engelmann, Elena V.M. Lieven, Julian M. Pine, Anna L. Theakston, and Ben Ambridge
Journal of Memory and Language, 2019, Volume 107, Page 169
[4]
BEN AMBRIDGE, EVAN KIDD, CAROLINE F. ROWLAND, and ANNA L. THEAKSTON
Journal of Child Language, 2015, Volume 42, Number 2, Page 239
[5]
Eglė SAVIČIŪTĖ, Ben AMBRIDGE, and Julian M. PINE
Journal of Child Language, 2018, Volume 45, Number 3, Page 641
[6]
Felix Engelmann, Sonia Granlund, Joanna Kolak, Marta Szreder, Ben Ambridge, Julian Pine, Anna Theakston, and Elena Lieven
Cognitive Psychology, 2019, Volume 110, Page 30
[7]
Sanna H. M. Räsänen, Ben Ambridge, and Julian M. Pine
Cognitive Science, 2016, Volume 40, Number 7, Page 1704
[8]
Ben Ambridge, Julian M. Pine, Caroline F. Rowland, Franklin Chang, and Amy Bidgood
Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 2013, Volume 4, Number 1, Page 47

Comments (0)

Please log in or register to comment.
Log in