Jump to ContentJump to Main Navigation
Show Summary Details

Cognitive Linguistics

Editor-in-Chief: Newman, John

4 Issues per year


IMPACT FACTOR increased in 2015: 1.375
5-year IMPACT FACTOR: 1.527
Rank 29 out of 179 in category Linguistics in the 2015 Thomson Reuters Journal Citation Report/Social Sciences Edition

SCImago Journal Rank (SJR) 2015: 0.592
Source Normalized Impact per Paper (SNIP) 2015: 1.277
Impact per Publication (IPP) 2015: 0.833

Online
ISSN
1613-3641
See all formats and pricing

 


Select Volume and Issue

Issues

30,00 € / $42.00 / £23.00

Get Access to Full Text

Frequency vs. iconicity in explaining grammatical asymmetries

Martin Haspelmath
  • Max-Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Leipzig, Germany.
  • :
Published Online: 2008-03-12 | DOI: https://doi.org/10.1515/COG.2008.001

Abstract

This paper argues that three widely accepted motivating factors subsumed under the broad heading of iconicity, namely iconicity of quantity, iconicity of complexity and iconicity of cohesion, in fact have no role in explaining grammatical asymmetries and should be discarded. The iconicity accounts of the relevant phenomena have been proposed by authorities like Jakobson, Haiman and Givón, but I argue that these linguists did not sufficiently consider alternative usage-based explanations in terms of frequency of use. A closer look shows that the well-known Zipfian effects of frequency of use (leading to shortness and fusion) can be made responsible for all of the alleged iconicity effects, and initial corpus data for a range of phenomena confirm the correctness of the approach.

Keywords: frequency; iconicity; markedness; economic motivation

*Contact address: Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Deutscher Platz 6, 04103 Leipzig, Germany


Received: 2006-12-04

Revised: 2007-04-05

Published Online: 2008-03-12

Published in Print: 2008-02-01


Citation Information: Cognitive Linguistics. Volume 19, Issue 1, Pages 1–33, ISSN (Online) 1613-3641, ISSN (Print) 0936-5907, DOI: https://doi.org/10.1515/COG.2008.001, March 2008

Citing Articles

Here you can find all Crossref-listed publications in which this article is cited. If you would like to receive automatic email messages as soon as this article is cited in other publications, simply activate the “Citation Alert” on the top of this page.

[1]
Marlou van Rijn
Transactions of the Philological Society, 2016, Page n/a
[3]
AMY ROSE DEAL
Journal of Linguistics, 2015, Page 1
[5]
Kasper Boye, Eva van Lier, and Eva Theilgaard Brink
Language Sciences, 2015, Volume 51, Page 1
[7]
Martin Haspelmath
Annual Review of Linguistics, 2015, Volume 1, Number 1, Page 19
[9]
Hanjung Lee
Discourse and Cognition, 2011, Volume 18, Number 3, Page 219
[10]
MARTIN HASPELMATH, ANDREEA CALUDE, MICHAEL SPAGNOL, HEIKO NARROG, and ELİF BAMYACI
Journal of Linguistics, 2014, Volume 50, Number 03, Page 587
[11]
P. Monaghan, R. C. Shillcock, M. H. Christiansen, and S. Kirby
Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 2014, Volume 369, Number 1651, Page 20130299
[13]
Jochen Trommer
Linguistic Inquiry, 2013, Volume 44, Number 1, Page 109
[14]
Victor Kuperman and Raymond Bertram
Language and Cognitive Processes, 2013, Volume 28, Number 7, Page 939
[15]
David Kummerow
Australian Journal of Linguistics, 2012, Volume 32, Number 2, Page 259
[17]
T. Florian Jaeger and Harry Tily
Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 2011, Volume 2, Number 3, Page 323
[18]
Martin Haspelmath
Theoretical Linguistics, 2011, Volume 37, Number 1-2
[19]
Sander Lestrade
Linguistics, 2010, Volume 48, Number 3
[21]
Hanjung Lee
Journal of East Asian Linguistics, 2010, Volume 19, Number 4, Page 291
[22]
THOMAS BERG
Journal of Linguistics, 2011, Volume 47, Number 01, Page 31
[23]
Florian Schäfer
Language and Linguistics Compass, 2009, Volume 3, Number 2, Page 641

Comments (0)

Please log in or register to comment.