Jump to ContentJump to Main Navigation
Show Summary Details

Cognitive Linguistics

Editor-in-Chief: Newman, John

4 Issues per year


IMPACT FACTOR increased in 2015: 1.375
5-year IMPACT FACTOR: 1.527
Rank 29 out of 179 in category Linguistics in the 2015 Thomson Reuters Journal Citation Report/Social Sciences Edition

SCImago Journal Rank (SJR) 2015: 0.592
Source Normalized Impact per Paper (SNIP) 2015: 1.277
Impact per Publication (IPP) 2015: 0.833

Online
ISSN
1613-3641
See all formats and pricing

 


Select Volume and Issue

Issues

30,00 € / $42.00 / £23.00

Get Access to Full Text

In defence of iconicity

John Haiman
  • Macalester College, USA.
  • :
Published Online: 2008-03-12 | DOI: https://doi.org/10.1515/COG.2008.002

Abstract

A number of iconically motivated grammatical distinctions, among them that between alienable and inalienable possession in Japanese and Korean, are graded. Haspelmath's Zipfian frequency hypothesis may be able to accommodate these facts (lowest bulk is most frequent, middle bulk is less frequent, and maximal bulk is maximally infrequent), but until more data are forthcoming, iconicity alone makes the correct predictions in those cases, and (crucially) in others where bulk is simply not the grammatical variable at issue in signaling markedness (as for example, the distinction between nominative/absolutive and ergative/accusative in Kurdish). The productivity (not just the fortuitous correctness) of an iconically motivated “more form” implies “more meaning” principle is attested in: (a) the (pre)history of the development of nominalizations in Romanian and Khmer, (b) in the frequent operation of “Watkins' Law” whereby 3sg. forms are interpreted as if they were zero-marked, even when they are not, and (c) grammaticality judgments about the differences between anaphoric epithets and structurally identical non-anaphoric noun phrases like the pig in English. Like reduced form, so too elaborated form, may have a number of motivations, not only iconic and economic (both cognitive), but also esthetic. It is probably misconceived to look for only one motivating factor to account for most observed grammatical facts, although the motivating factors are more easily identified when they operate alone.

Keywords: iconicity; frequency; productivity

*Contact address: Linguistics Search, Macalester College, 1600 Grand Avenue, St. Paul, MN 55105, USA.


Received: 2007-02-28

Published Online: 2008-03-12

Published in Print: 2008-02-01


Citation Information: Cognitive Linguistics. Volume 19, Issue 1, Pages 35–48, ISSN (Online) 1613-3641, ISSN (Print) 0936-5907, DOI: https://doi.org/10.1515/COG.2008.002, March 2008

Citing Articles

Here you can find all Crossref-listed publications in which this article is cited. If you would like to receive automatic email messages as soon as this article is cited in other publications, simply activate the “Citation Alert” on the top of this page.

[1]
Michael Fortescue
International Journal of American Linguistics, 2014, Volume 80, Number 4, Page 533
[2]
Victor Kuperman and Raymond Bertram
Language and Cognitive Processes, 2013, Volume 28, Number 7, Page 939
[4]
John Haiman
Linguistics, 2010, Volume 48, Number 3

Comments (0)

Please log in or register to comment.