Jump to ContentJump to Main Navigation
Show Summary Details
More options …

Cognitive Linguistics

Editor-in-Chief: Newman, John

4 Issues per year


IMPACT FACTOR 2016: 2.135

CiteScore 2016: 1.29

SCImago Journal Rank (SJR) 2016: 1.247
Source Normalized Impact per Paper (SNIP) 2016: 1.485

Online
ISSN
1613-3641
See all formats and pricing
More options …
Volume 27, Issue 2

Issues

When do language comprehenders mentally simulate locations?

Nian Liu
  • Corresponding author
  • Department of Modern Languages, Literatures, and Linguistics, University of Oklahoma, Norman, OK 73019, USA
  • Email
  • Other articles by this author:
  • De Gruyter OnlineGoogle Scholar
/ Benjamin Bergen
Published Online: 2016-03-12 | DOI: https://doi.org/10.1515/cog-2015-0123

Abstract

Embodied approaches to comprehension propose that understanding language entails performing mental simulations of its content. The evidence, however, is mixed. Action-sentence Compatibility Effect studies (Glenberg and Kaschak 2002) report mental simulation of motor actions during processing of motion language. But the same studies find no evidence that language comprehenders perform spatial simulations of the corresponding locations. This challenges simulation-based approaches. If locations are not represented in simulation, but are still understood, then simulation may be unnecessary for understanding. We conducted a Location-sentence Compatibility experiment, to determine whether understanders mentally simulate locations. People did indeed simulate locations, but only when sentences used progressive (and not perfect) grammatical aspect. Moreover, mental simulations of locations differed for language about concrete versus abstract events. These findings substantiate the role of mental simulation in language understanding, while highlighting the importance of the grammatical form of utterances as well as their content.

Keywords: embodiment; mental simulation; location; grammatical aspect; abstractness

References

  • Anderson, Sarah, Teenie Matlock & Michael J. Spivey. 2010. The role of grammatical aspect in the dynamics of spatial descriptions. In Cristoph Hölscher, Thomas F. Shipley, Marta Olivetti Belardinelli, John A. Bateman & Nora S. Newcombe (eds.), Spatial cognition, Vol. 7 (Lecture Notes in Artificial Intelligence 6222), 139–151. Berlin: Springer-Verlag.Google Scholar

  • Barsalou, Lawrence W. 1999. Perceptual symbol systems. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 22. 577–609.Google Scholar

  • Barsalou, Lawrence W. & Katja Wiemer-Hastings. 2005. Situating abstract concepts. In Diane Pecher & Rolf A. Zwaan (eds.), Grounding cognition: The role of perception and action in memory, language, and thought, 129–163. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar

  • Bergen, Benjamin K. 2007. Experimental methods for simulation semantics. In Monica Gonzalez-Marquez, Irene Mittelberg, Seana Coulson & Michael J. Spivey (eds.), Methods in cognitive linguistics, 277–301. Amsterdam & Philadelphia: John Benjamins.Google Scholar

  • Bergen, Benjamin K. & Nancy Chang. 2005. Embodied construction grammar in simulation-based language understanding. In Jan-Ola Östman & Mirjam Fried (eds.), Construction grammar(s): Cognitive grounding and theoretical extensions, 147–190. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar

  • Bergen, Benjamin K., Shane Lindsay, Teenie Matlock & Srini Narayanan. 2007. Spatial and linguistic aspects of visual imagery in sentence comprehension. Cognitive Science 31, 733–764.Google Scholar

  • Bergen, Benjamin K., Shweta Narayan & Jerome Feldman. 2003. Embodied verbal semantics: Evidence from an image-verb matching task. In Richard Alterman & David Kirsh (eds.), Proceedings of the Twenty-Fifth Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, 139–144. Boston, MA: Cognitive Science Society.Google Scholar

  • Bergen, Benjamin K. & Kathryn B. Wheeler. 2005. Sentence understanding engages motor processes. In Bruno G. Bara, Lawrence W. Barsalou & Monica Bucciarelli (eds.), Proceedings of the Twenty-Seventh Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society, 238–243. New York: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar

  • Bergen, Benjamin K. & Kathryn B. Wheeler. 2010. Grammatical aspect and mental simulation. Brain and Language 112. 150–158.Google Scholar

  • Brunyé, Tad T., Tali Ditman, Caroline R. Mahoney, Jason S. Augustyn & Holly A. Taylor. 2009. When you and I share perspectives: Pronouns and perspective-taking during narrative comprehension. Psychological Science 20(1). 27–32.Google Scholar

  • Bub, Daniel N., Michael E. J. Masson & George S. Cree. 2008. Evocation of functional and volumetric gestural knowledge by objects and words. Cognition 106. 27–58.Google Scholar

  • Carreiras, Manuel, Núria Carriedo, María Angeles Alonso & Angel Fernández. 1997. The role of verb tense and verb aspect in the foregrounding of information during reading. Memory and Cognition 25. 438–446.Google Scholar

  • Chomsky, Noam. 1957. Syntactic structures. The Hague & Paris: Mouton.Google Scholar

  • Comrie, Bernard. 1976. Aspect. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar

  • Connell, Louise. 2007. Representing object color in language comprehension. Cognition 102. 476–485.Google Scholar

  • Dowty, David R. 1977. Toward a semantic analysis of verb aspect and the English “imperfective” progressive. Linguistics and Philosophy 1. 45–77.Google Scholar

  • Feldman, Jerome & Srinivas Narayanan. 2004. Embodied meaning in a neural theory of language. Brain and Language 89. 385–392.Google Scholar

  • Ferretti, Todd R., Marta Kutas & Ken McRae. 2007. Verb aspect and the activation of event knowledge. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 33(1). 182–196.Google Scholar

  • Fodor, Jerry A. 1983. The modularity of mind: An essay on faculty psychology. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar

  • Gallese, Vittorio & George Lakoff. 2005. The brain’s concepts: The role of the sensory-motor system in reason and language. Cognitive Neuropsychology 22. 455–479.Google Scholar

  • Giora, Rachel, Noga Balaban, Ofer Fein & Inbar Alkabets. 2004. Negation as positivity in disguise. In Herbert L. Colston & Albert Katz (eds.), Figurative language comprehension: Social and cultural influences, 233–258. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar

  • Glenberg, Arthur M., Raymond Becker, Susann Klötzer, Lidia Kolanko, Silvana Müller & Mike Rinck. 2009. Episodic affordances contribute to language comprehension. Language and Cognition 1. 113–135.Google Scholar

  • Glenberg, Arthur & Michael Kaschak. 2002. Grounding language in action. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review 9. 558–565.Google Scholar

  • Goldberg, Adele E. 1995. Constructions: A construction grammar approach to argument structure. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar

  • Guan, Connie Qun, Wanjin Meng, Ru Yao & Arthur M. Glenberg. 2013. The motor system contributes to comprehension of abstract language. PloS one 8(9). e75183. DOI 10.1371/journal.pone.0075183.Google Scholar

  • Kaschak, Michael P., Carol J. Madden, David J. Therriault, Richard H. Yaxley, Mark Aveyard, Adrienne A. Blanchard & Rolf A. Zwaan. 2005. Perception of motion affects language processing. Cognition 94. B79–B89.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kaup, Barbara, Jana Lüdtke & Rolf A. Zwaan. 2007. The experiential view of language comprehension: How is negated information represented? In Franz Schmalhofer & Charles A. Perfetti (eds.), Higher level language processes in the brain: Inference and comprehension processes, 255–288. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar

  • Kosslyn, Stephen M., Giorgio Ganis & William L. Thompson. 2001. Neural foundations of imagery. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 2. 635–642.Google Scholar

  • Lakoff, George & Mark Johnson. 1980. Metaphors we live by. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar

  • Langacker, Ronald W. 1983. Remarks on English aspect. In Paul J. Hopper (ed.), Tense and aspect: Between semantics and pragmatics, 265–304. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar

  • Madden, Carol J. & David J. Therriault. 2009. Verb aspect and perceptual simulations. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 62(7). 1294–1302.Google Scholar

  • Madden, Carol J. & Rolf A. Zwaan. 2003. How does verb aspect constrain event representations? Memory and Cognition 31. 663–672.Google Scholar

  • Magliano, Joseph P. & Michelle C. Schleich. 2000. Verb aspect and situation models. Discourse Processes 29. 83–112.Google Scholar

  • Matlock, Teenie. 2011. The conceptual motivation of aspect. In Klaus-Uwe Panther & Günther Radden (eds.), Motivation in grammar and the lexicon, 133–147. Amsterdam & Philadelphia: John Benjamins.Google Scholar

  • Pecher, Diane, René Zeelenberg & Lawrence W. Barsalou. 2003. Verifying properties from different modalities for concepts produces switching costs. Psychological Science 14. 119–124.Google Scholar

  • Richardson, Daniel C. & Teenie Matlock. 2007. The integration of figurative language and static depictions: An eye movement study of fictive motion. Cognition 102. 129–138.Google Scholar

  • Richardson, Daniel C., Michael J. Spivey, Lawrence W. Barsalou & Ken McRae. 2003. Spatial representations activated during real-time comprehension of verbs. Cognitive Science 27. 767–780.Google Scholar

  • Solomon, Karen O. & Lawrence W. Barsalou. 2004. Perceptual simulation in property verification. Memory and Cognition 32. 244–259.Google Scholar

  • Stanfield, Robert A. & Rolf A. Zwaan. 2001. The effect of implied orientation derived from verbal context on picture recognition. Psychological Science 12. 153–156.Google Scholar

  • Taylor, Lawrence J. & Rolf A. Zwaan. 2008. Motor resonance and linguistic focus. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 61. 869–904.Google Scholar

  • Wheeler, Mark E., Steven E. Petersen & Randy L. Buckner. 2000. Memory’s echo: Vivid remembering reactivates sensory-specific cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 97. 11125–11129.Google Scholar

  • Zacks, Jeff M. & Barbara Tversky. 2001. Event structure in perception and conception. Psychological Bulletin 127(1). 3–21.Google Scholar

  • Zwaan, Rolf A. 1999. Embodied cognition, perceptual symbols, and situation models. Discourse Processes 28. 81–88.Google Scholar

  • Zwaan, Rolf A., Robert A. Stanfield & Richard H. Yaxley. 2002. Language comprehenders mentally represent the shapes of objects. Psychological Science 13. 168–171.Google Scholar

About the article

Received: 2015-11-26

Revised: 2015-12-13

Accepted: 2015-12-13

Published Online: 2016-03-12

Published in Print: 2016-05-01


Citation Information: Cognitive Linguistics, Volume 27, Issue 2, Pages 181–203, ISSN (Online) 1613-3641, ISSN (Print) 0936-5907, DOI: https://doi.org/10.1515/cog-2015-0123.

Export Citation

©2016 by De Gruyter Mouton. Copyright Clearance Center

Citing Articles

Here you can find all Crossref-listed publications in which this article is cited. If you would like to receive automatic email messages as soon as this article is cited in other publications, simply activate the “Citation Alert” on the top of this page.

[1]
Anita Eerland, Andrew M. Sherrill, Joseph P. Magliano, and Rolf A. Zwaan
Collabra: Psychology, 2017, Volume 3, Number 1, Page 29

Comments (0)

Please log in or register to comment.
Log in