Jump to ContentJump to Main Navigation
Show Summary Details
More options …

HUMOR

International Journal of Humor Research

Editor-in-Chief: Ford, Thomas E.

4 Issues per year


IMPACT FACTOR 2017: 0.660
5-year IMPACT FACTOR: 1.059

CiteScore 2017: 1.27

SCImago Journal Rank (SJR) 2017: 0.415
Source Normalized Impact per Paper (SNIP) 2017: 1.228

Online
ISSN
1613-3722
See all formats and pricing
More options …
Volume 28, Issue 2

Issues

Put-Down Humor Directed at Outgroup Members Increases Perceived – but Not Experienced – Cohesion in Groups

Christine Gockel / Norbert L. Kerr
Published Online: 2015-04-02 | DOI: https://doi.org/10.1515/humor-2015-0020

Abstract

Put-down humor always has a target, and the intent of the speaker is to make fun of someone or something. Due to its inherently social nature, put-down humor could strongly affect socio-emotional group processes and outcomes like cohesion. We hypothesized that put-down humor would increase cohesion when it was targeted at outgroup members and was followed by laughter. Two laboratory experiments were conducted to examine the underlying processes of the put-down humor – cohesion relation. Study 1 showed that put-down humor of outgroup members (as compared to no humor) did not increase participants’ attraction to the group. But it increased perceived cohesion among other group members and the perception that other group members felt safer and were more similar to each other. Even when procedures were altered in Study 2 to reduce participants’ evaluation apprehension and their potential anxiety about not themselves being humorous, we found the same pattern of results. We discuss which conditions might be necessary for put-down humor to increase one’s personal attraction to a group.

Keywords: put-down humor; group; cohesion; positive mood; psychological safety

References

  • Baron, R. M. & Kenny, D. A. 1986. The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal of Personal and Social Psychology 51. 1173–1182.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Berk, R. A. 2001. The active ingredients in humor: Psychophysiological benefits and risks for older adults. Educational Gerontology 27. 323–339.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bollen, K. A. & Hoyle, R. H. 1990. Perceived cohesion: A conceptual and empirical examination. Social Forces 69. 479–504.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Byrne, D. 1971. Can Wright be wrong? Let me count the ways. Representative Research in Social Psychology 2. 12–18.Google Scholar

  • Coser, R. L. 1959. Some social functions of laughter. Human Relations 12. 171–182.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Dunbar, N., Banas, J., Rodriguez, D., Liu, S., & Abra, G. 2012. Humor use in power-differentiated interactions. Humor 25. 469–489.Web of ScienceGoogle Scholar

  • Duncan, W. J., & Feisal, J. P. 1989. No laughing matter: Patterns of humor in the workplace. Organizational Dynamics 17. 18–30.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Edmondson, A. 1999. Psychological safety and learning behavior in work teams. Administrative Science Quarterly 44. 350–383.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Eisenberg, A. R. 1986. Teasing: Verbal play in two Mexicano homes. In B. B. Schieffelin & E. Ochs (eds.), Languages socialization across cultures, 183–198. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar

  • Fine, G. A., & De Soucey, M. 2005. Joking cultures: Humor themes as social regulation in group life. Humor 18. 1–22.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Foot, H. 1991. The psychology of humor and laughter. In R. Cochrane & D. Carroll (eds.), Psychology and social issues: A tutorial text, 1–13. London & New York: Falmer Press.Google Scholar

  • Forgas, J. P., & Bower, G. H. 1987. Mood effects on person perception judgments. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 53. 53–60.PubMedCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Fraley, B., & Aron, A. 2004. The effect of a shared humorous experience on closeness in initial encounters. Personal Relationships 11. 61–78.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Francis, L., Monahan, K., & Berger, C. 1999. A laughing matter? The uses of humor in medical interactions. Motivation and Emotion 23. 155–174.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gouaux, C. 1971. Induced affective states and interpersonal attraction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 20. 37–43.CrossrefPubMedGoogle Scholar

  • Kerr, N. L. 2012. Reflections on the “Fourth Drawback” [Blog post]. http://grouplab.wordpress.com/2013/03/13/the-kerrmudgeon-blog-pet-peeves-of-a-social-psychological-greybeard/

  • La Fave, L., Haddad, J., & Maeson, W. A. 1976. Superiority, enhanced self-esteem and perceived incongruity humor theory. In A. J. Chapman & H. C. Foot (eds.), Humor and laughter: Theory, research and applications. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar

  • Levine, J. 1969. Motivation in humor. New York: Atherton Press.Google Scholar

  • Long, D. L., & Graesser, A. C. 1988. Wit and humor in discourse processing. Discourse Processes 11(1). 35–60.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • MacCoun, R. J. 1993. What is known about unit cohesion and military performance. In Sexual orientation and U.S. military personnel policy: Options and assessment, 283–331. Santa Monica, CA: RAND.Google Scholar

  • Martin, R. A. 2001. Humor, laughter, and physical health: Methodological issues and research findings. Psychological Bulletin 127. 504–519.CrossrefPubMedGoogle Scholar

  • Pogebrin, M. R., & Poole, E. D. 1988. Humor in the briefing room: A study of the strategic uses of humor among police. Journal of Contemporary Ethnography 17. 183–210.Google Scholar

  • Platt, T. & Forabosco, G. 2012. Gelotophobia: The fear of being laughed at. In P. Gremigni (ed.), Humor and health promotion, 229–252. Hauppauge, NY: Nova Biomedical Books.Google Scholar

  • Preacher, K. J., & Hayes, A. F. 2008. Asymptotic and resampling strategies for assessing and comparing indirect effects in multiple mediator models. Behavior Research Methods 40. 879–891.PubMedCrossrefWeb of ScienceGoogle Scholar

  • Robinson, D. T., & Smith-Lovin, L. 2001. Getting a laugh: Gender, status, and humor in task discussions. Social Forces 80. 123–158.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Romero, E. & Cruthirds, K. 2006. The use of humor in the workplace. Academy of Management Perspectives 20. 58–69.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Ruch, W. 1992. Assessment of appreciation of humor: Studies with the 3 WD humor test. In C. D. Spielberger & J. N. Butcher (eds.), Advances in personality assessment, Vol. 9, 27–75. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar

  • Ruch, W., & Forabosco, G. 1996. A cross-cultural study of humor appreciation – Italy and Germany. Humor-International Journal of Humor Research 9. 1–18.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Sobel, M. E. 1982. Asymptotic confidence intervals for indirect effects in structural equation models. In S. Leinhardt (ed.), Sociological methodology 1982, 290–312. Washington, DC: American Sociological Association.Google Scholar

  • Terrion, J. L., & Ashforth, B. E. 2002. From “I” to “we”: The role of putdown humor and identity in the development of a temporary group. Human Relations 55. 55–88.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Tragesser, S. L., & Lippman, L. G. 2005. Teasing: For superiority or solidarity? The Journal of General Psychology 132. 255–266.CrossrefPubMedGoogle Scholar

  • Van den Broeck, A., Vander Elst, T., Dikkers, J., De Lange, A., & De Witte, H. 2012. This is funny: On the beneficial role of self-enhancing and affiliative humour in job design. Psicothema 24. 87–93.PubMedGoogle Scholar

  • Veitch, R., & Griffit, W. 1976. Good news – bad news: Affective and interpersonal effects. Journal of Applied Social Psychology 6. 69–75.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Vinton, K. L. 1989. Humor in the workplace: It is more than telling jokes. Small Group Behavior 20. 151–166.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. 1988. Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 54. 1063–1070.PubMedCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wyer, R. S. Jr., & Collins, J. E., II. 1992. A theory of humor elicitation. Psychological Review 99. 663–688.PubMedCrossrefGoogle Scholar

About the article

Christine Gockel

Christine Gockel is Professor of Business Psychology at SRH University of Applied Sciences Berlin. Prior to that, she held post-doctoral positions at Chemnitz University of Technology and the University of Fribourg, after receiving her Ph.D. in 2007 at Michigan State University. In her research, she focuses on transactive memory systems, shared leadership, and humor in teams.

Norbert L. Kerr

Norbert L. Kerr is Professor of Social Psychology at the University of Kent and Emeritus Professor of Psychology at Michigan State University. His research interests focus on group performance, social dilemmas, and psychology and the law.


Published Online: 2015-04-02

Published in Print: 2015-05-01


Citation Information: HUMOR, Volume 28, Issue 2, Pages 205–228, ISSN (Online) 1613-3722, ISSN (Print) 0933-1719, DOI: https://doi.org/10.1515/humor-2015-0020.

Export Citation

©2015 by De Gruyter Mouton.Get Permission

Citing Articles

Here you can find all Crossref-listed publications in which this article is cited. If you would like to receive automatic email messages as soon as this article is cited in other publications, simply activate the “Citation Alert” on the top of this page.

[1]
Gillian Hendry, Sally Wiggins, and Tony Anderson
Psychology Learning & Teaching, 2016, Volume 15, Number 2, Page 180
[2]
Laura Vetter and Christine Gockel
Gruppe. Interaktion. Organisation. Zeitschrift für Angewandte Organisationspsychologie (GIO), 2016, Volume 47, Number 4, Page 313

Comments (0)

Please log in or register to comment.
Log in