Jump to ContentJump to Main Navigation
Show Summary Details
More options …

Journal of Causal Inference

Ed. by Imai, Kosuke / Pearl, Judea / Petersen, Maya Liv / Sekhon, Jasjeet / van der Laan, Mark J.

Online
ISSN
2193-3685
See all formats and pricing
More options …

The Entry of Randomized Assignment into the Social Sciences

Julian C. JamisonORCID iD: https://orcid.org/0000-0003-2671-1153
Published Online: 2019-03-22 | DOI: https://doi.org/10.1515/jci-2017-0025

Abstract

Although the concept of randomized assignment in order to control for extraneous confounding factors reaches back hundreds of years, the first empirical use appears to have been in an 1835 trial of homeopathic medicine. Throughout the 19th century there was a growing awareness of the need for comparison groups, albeit often without the realization that randomization could be a clean method to achieve that goal. In the second and more crucial phase of this history, four separate but related disciplines introduced randomized control trials within a few years of one another in the 1920s: agricultural science; clinical medicine; educational psychology; and social policy (specifically political science). This brought increasing rigor to fields that were focusing more on causal relationships. In a third phase, the 1950s through 1970s saw a surge of interest in more applied randomized experiments in economics and elsewhere – both in the lab and especially in the field.

Keywords: randomization; RCT; field experiment; lab experiment; confounding; causality; history of science

References

  • 1.

    Pearl J. Causality. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 2000.Google Scholar

  • 2.

    Smith GCS, Pell JP. Parachute use to prevent death and major trauma related to gravitational challenge: systematic review of randomised controlled trials. Br Med J. 2003;327:1459–61.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • 3.

    Chalmers I. Comparing like with like: some historical milestones in the evolution of methods to create unbiased comparison groups in therapeutic experiments. Int J Epidemiol. 2001;30:1156–64.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • 4.

    Fienberg SE, Tanur JM. Experimental and sampling structures: parallels diverging and meeting. Int Stat Rev. 1987;55(1):75–96.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • 5.

    Mahalanobis PC. Recent experiments in statistical sampling in the Indian Statistical Institute. J R Stat Soc. 1946;109:325–78.Google Scholar

  • 6.

    Urbach P. Randomization and the design of experiments. Philos Sci. 1985;52:256–73.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • 7.

    Papineau D. The virtues of randomization. Br J Philos Sci. 1994;45(2):437–50.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • 8.

    Silverman WA, Chalmers I. Casting and drawing lots: a time honoured way of dealing with uncertainty and ensuring fairness. Br Med J. 2001;323:1467–8.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • 9.

    van Helmont JB. Ortus Medicinæ. Id Est, Initia Physicæ Inaudita. Amsterdam: Elsevier; 1648.Google Scholar

  • 10.

    Fisher RA. The design of experiments. London: Oliver and Boyd; 1935.Google Scholar

  • 11.

    Eden T, Fisher RA. Studies in crop variation, IV. The experimental determination of the value of top dressings with cereals. J Agric Sci. 1927;17:548–62.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • 12.

    Darwin C. On the origin of species by means of natural selection. London: John Murray; 1859.Google Scholar

  • 13.

    Petrarch J. Letter to Boccaccio (V.3). Rerum Senilium Libri. Liber XIV: Epistola 1. 1364.

  • 14.

    van Helmont JB. Oriatrike, or physick refined: the common errors therein refuted and the whole art reformed and rectified London: Lodowick-Loyd; 1662.Google Scholar

  • 15.

    Lind J. A treatise of the scurvy. In three parts. Containing an inquiry into the nature, causes and cure, of that disease. Together with a critical and chronological view of what has been published on the Subject Edinburgh: Kincaid and Donaldson; 1753.Google Scholar

  • 16.

    Watson W. An account of a series of experiments, instituted with a view of ascertaining the most successful method of inoculating the smallpox. London: J Nourse; 1768.Google Scholar

  • 17.

    Pasteur L. Compte-rendu Sommaire des Expériences Faites à Pouilly-le-Fort près Melun, sur la Vaccination Charbonneuse. C R Acad Sci. 1881;92:1378–83.Google Scholar

  • 18.

    Claassen JAHR. The gold standard: not a golden standard. Br Med J. 2005;330:1121.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • 19.

    Löhner G. Die Homöopathischen Kochsalzversuche zu Nürnberg. Nuremberg. 1835.Google Scholar

  • 20.

    Stolberg M. Inventing the randomized double-blind trial. J R Soc Med. 2006;99:642–3.Google Scholar

  • 21.

    Kaptchuk TJ. Intentional ignorance: a history of blind assessment and placebo controls in medicine. Bull Hist Med. 1998;72(3):389–433.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • 22.

    Balfour TG. as quoted in West C. Lectures on the diseases of infancy and childhood. London: Longman Brown Green and Longmans; 1854.Google Scholar

  • 23.

    Fibiger J. Om Serumbehandling af Difteri. Hospitalstidende. 1898;6:309–25.Google Scholar

  • 24.

    Colebrook D. Irradiation and health. Med Res Coun Spec Rep. 1929;131:4–13.Google Scholar

  • 25.

    Doull JA, Hardy M, Clark JH, Herman MB. The effect of irradiation with ultra-violet light on the frequency of attacks of upper respiratory disease. Am J Hyg. 1931;13:460–77.Google Scholar

  • 26.

    Marks HM. James angus doull and the well-controlled common cold. J R Soc Med. 2008;101(10):117–9.Google Scholar

  • 27.

    Medical Research Council. Streptomycin treatment of pulmonary tuberculosis: a medical research council investigation. Br Med J. 1948;2:769–82.Google Scholar

  • 28.

    Medical Research Council. Prevention of whooping-cough by vaccination: a medical research council investigation. Br Med J. 1951;1:1463–71.Google Scholar

  • 29.

    Fechner G. Elemente der Psychophysik. Leipzig: von Breitkopf & Haertel; 1860.Google Scholar

  • 30.

    Müller GE. Über die Maassbestimmungen des Ortsinnes der Haut Mittels der Methode der Richtigen un Falschen Fälle. Arch Gesammte Physiologie Menschen Thiere. 1879;19:191–235.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • 31.

    Dehue T. Deception, efficiency, and random groups: psychology and the gradual origination of the random group design. Isis. 1997;88(4):653–73.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • 32.

    Richet C. La Suggestion Mentale et le Calcul des Probabilités. Rev Philos Fr étrang. 1884;18:609–74.Google Scholar

  • 33.

    Hacking I. Telepathy: origins of randomization in experimental design. Isis. 1988;79(3):427–51.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • 34.

    Stigler SM. A historical view of statistical concepts in psychology and educational research. Am J Educ. 1992;101:60–70.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • 35.

    Peirce CS, Jastrow J. On small differences of sensation. Mem Natl Acad Sci 1884. 1885;3:75–83.Google Scholar

  • 36.

    Forsetlund L, Chalmers I, Bjørndal A. When was random allocation first used to generate comparison groups in experiments to assess the effects of social interventions? Econ Innov New Technol. 2007;16(5):371–84.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • 37.

    McCall WA. How to experiment in education. New York: Macmillan; 1923.Google Scholar

  • 38.

    Dearborn WF, Lincoln EA. A class experiment in learning. J Educ Psychol. 1922;13(6):330–40.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • 39.

    Shaffer LF. A learning experiment in the social studies. J Educ Psychol. 1927;18(9):577–91.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • 40.

    Clark BE. The effect upon retention of varying lengths of study periods and rest intervals in distributed learning time. J Educ Psychol. 1928;19(8):552–9.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • 41.

    Thurstone LL. The indifference function. J Soc Psychol. 1931;2:139–67.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • 42.

    Roth AE. On the early history of experimental economics. J Hist Econ Thought. 1993;15:184–209.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • 43.

    Davidson D, Siegel S, Suppes P. Some experiments and related theory on the measurement of utility and subjective probability. Office of Naval Research Contract NR 171-034 Technical Report 1. 15 August 1955.

  • 44.

    Chamberlin EH. An experimental imperfect market. J Polit Econ. 1948;56(2):95–108.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • 45.

    Smith VL. An experimental study of competitive market behavior. J Polit Econ. 1962;70(2):111–37.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • 46.

    Kalisch G, Milnor JW, Nash J, Nering ED. Some experimental n-person games. RAND Research Memorandum 948, Aug 25 1952.

  • 47.

    Atkinson RC, Suppes P. An analysis of two-person game situations in terms of statistical learning theory. Office of Naval Research Contract NR 171-034 Technical Report 8. 25 April 1957.

  • 48.

    Searle B, Matthews P, Suppes P, Friend J. Formal evaluation of the 1976 first-grade instructional program. In: Suppes P, Searle B, Friend J, editors. The radio mathematics project: Nicaragua 1976–77. Stanford CA: Institute for Mathematical Studies in the Social Sciences; 1978. p. 97–124.Google Scholar

  • 49.

    Siegel S, Fouraker LE. Bargaining and group decision-making: experiments in bilateral monopoly. New York: McGraw-Hill; 1960.Google Scholar

  • 50.

    Suppes P, Carlsmith JM. Experimental analysis of a duopoly situation from the standpoint of mathematical learning theory. Int Econ Rev. 1962;3(1):60–78.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • 51.

    Smith VL. Discovery – a memoir Bloomington: AuthorHouse; 2008.Google Scholar

  • 52.

    Logan CH. Evaluation research in crime and delinquency: a reappraisal. J Crim Law Criminol. 1973;63(3):378–87.Google Scholar

  • 53.

    Boruch RF, McSweeny AJ, Soderstrom EJ. Randomized field experiments for program planning, development, and evaluation. Eval Q. 1978;2(4):655–95.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • 54.

    Farrington DP. Randomized experiments on crime and justice. Crime Justice. 1983;4:257–308.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • 55.

    Oakley A. A historical perspective on the use of randomized trials in social science settings. Crim Delinq. 2000;46(3):315–29.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • 56.

    Greenberg D, Shroder M. The digest of social experiments. 3rd ed. Washington DC: Urban Institute Press; 2004.Google Scholar

  • 57.

    Levitt SD, List JA. Field experiments in economics: the past, the present, and the future. Eur Econ Rev. 2009;53:1–18.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • 58.

    Gueron JM, Rolston H. Fighting for reliable evidence. New York: Russell Sage Foundation; 2013.Google Scholar

  • 59.

    Gosnell HF. Getting out the vote: an experiment in the stimulation of voting. Chicago: University of Chicago Press; 1927.Google Scholar

  • 60.

    Gosnell HF. An experiment in the stimulation of voting. Am Polit Sci Rev. 1926;20(4):869–74.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • 61.

    Walters JE. Seniors as counselors. J High Educ. 1931;2:446–8.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • 62.

    Eldersveld SJ. Experimental propaganda techniques and voting behavior. Am Polit Sci Rev. 1956;50:154–65.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • 63.

    Green DP, Gerber AS. The underprovision of experiments in political science. Ann Am Acad Polit Soc Sci. 2003;589:94–112.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • 64.

    Hartmann GW. A field experiment on the comparative effectiveness of ‘emotional’ and ‘rational’ political leaflets in determining election results. J Abnorm Soc Psychol. 1936;31(1):99–114.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • 65.

    Fiorina MP, Plott CR. Committee decisions under majority rule: an experimental study. Am Polit Sci Rev. 1978;72(2):575–98.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • 66.

    List JA, Rasul I. Field experiments in labor economics. In: Ashenfelter O, Card D, editors. Handbook of labor economics Vol 4a. Amsterdam: North Holland; 2011. p. 103–228.Google Scholar

  • 67.

    Powers E, Witmer H. An experiment in the prevention of juvenile delinquency: the Cambridge-Somerville Youth Study. New York: Columbia University Press; 1951.Google Scholar

  • 68.

    Reimer E, Warren M. Special intensive parole unit. Natl Probat Parole Assoc J. 1957;3:222–9.Google Scholar

  • 69.

    Hanson EH, Marks ES. Influence of the interviewer on the accuracy of survey results. J Am Stat Assoc. 1958;53:283. 635–55.Google Scholar

  • 70.

    Ares CE, Rankin A, Sturz H. The Manhattan Bail Project: an interim report on the use of pre-trial parole. NY Univ Law Rev. 1963;38:67–95.Google Scholar

  • 71.

    Francis T Jr, et al.. An evaluation of the 1954 poliomyelitis vaccine trials. Am J Publ Health. 1955;45(5(pt2):1–63.Google Scholar

  • 72.

    Meier P. The biggest public health experiment ever: the 1954 field trial of the salk poliomyelitis vaccine. In: Statistics: a guide to the unknown San Francisco: Holden-Day; 1972. p. 2–13.Google Scholar

  • 73.

    Council P. The taichung program of pre-pregnancy health. Stud Fam Plann. 1963;1(1):10–2.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • 74.

    Takeshita J. The taichung program of pre-pregnancy health. Stud Fam Plann. 1964;1(4):10–2.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • 75.

    Chang MC, Cernada GP, Sun TH. A field-worker incentive experimental study. Stud Fam Plann. 1972;3(11):270–2.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • 76.

    Campbell DT. Reforms as experiments. Am Psychol. 1969;24:409–29.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • 77.

    Deci EL. Effects of externally mediated rewards on intrinsic motivation. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1971;18(1):105–15.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • 78.

    Ross H. An experimental study of the negative income tax. Child Welf. 1970;49(10):562–9.Google Scholar

  • 79.

    Brook RH, et al.. Does free care improve adults’ health? – Results from a randomized controlled trial. N Engl J Med. 1983;309:1426–34.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • 80.

    Bohm P. Estimating demand for public goods: an experiment. Eur Econ Rev. 1972;3:111–30.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • 81.

    Hollister GH, Kemper P, Maynard RA. The national supported work demonstration. Madison, WI: Univ of Wisconsin Press; 1984.Google Scholar

  • 82.

    Jamison DT. Radio education and student failure in Nicaragua: a further note. In: Friend J, Searle B, Suppes P, editors. Radio mathematics in Nicaragua. Institute for Mathematical Studies in the Social Sciences: Stanford CA; 1980. p. 225–36.Google Scholar

  • 83.

    Harrison GW, List JA. Field experiments. J Econ Lit. 2004;42:1009–55.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • 84.

    Deaton A. Instruments, randomization, and learning about development. J Econ Lit. 2010;48:424–55.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • 85.

    Harrison GW. Field experiments and methodological intolerance. J Econ Methodol. 2013;20(2):103–17.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • 86.

    Kramer M, Shapiro S. Scientific challenges in the application of randomized trials. J Am Med Assoc. 1984;252(19):2739–45.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • 87.

    Harrison GW, Lau MI, Rutström. Rick attitudes, randomization to treatment, and self-selection into experiments. J Econ Behav Organ. 2009;70:498–507.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • 88.

    Gazzale R, Jamison JC, Karlan A, Karlan D. Ambiguous solicitation: ambiguous prescription. Econ Inq. 2013;51(1):1002–11.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

About the article

Received: 2017-11-04

Revised: 2019-01-20

Accepted: 2019-02-11

Published Online: 2019-03-22

Published in Print: 2019-04-26


Citation Information: Journal of Causal Inference, Volume 7, Issue 1, 20170025, ISSN (Online) 2193-3685, DOI: https://doi.org/10.1515/jci-2017-0025.

Export Citation

© 2019 Walter de Gruyter GmbH, Berlin/Boston.Get Permission

Comments (0)

Please log in or register to comment.
Log in