Jump to ContentJump to Main Navigation
Show Summary Details
More options …
New journal!

Open Information Science

Editor-in-Chief: Sturges, Paul

1 Issue per year

Open Access
Online
ISSN
2451-1781
See all formats and pricing
More options …

Ritual Artifacts as Symbolic Habits

Lorenzo Magnani
  • Department of Humanities, Philosophy Section and Computational Philosophy Laboratory, University of Pavia, Pavia, Italy
  • Email
  • Other articles by this author:
  • De Gruyter OnlineGoogle Scholar
Published Online: 2018-11-10 | DOI: https://doi.org/10.1515/opis-2018-0011

Abstract

The concepts of manipulative abduction and of extending, disembodying, and distributing the mind can help delineate important aspects of the role of habits, rituals, and symbols in human cognition, including when new concepts are created. Taking advantage of some psychoanalytical and anthropological issues, I will show how symbolic habits in rituals can function as memory mediators which are able to play significant roles in human cognition and action. They can maximize abducibility and so recoverability of knowledge contents, including at the unconscious level.

Keywords: Habits; rituals; symbols; Jung; abduction; memory mediators

References

  • Bertolotti T., L. Magnani. 2017. Theoretical considerations on cognitive niche construction. Synthese, 12(194):4757-4779.Google Scholar

  • Gibson J. J. 1979.The Ecological Approach to Visual Perception. Houghton Mifflin, Boston, MA.Google Scholar

  • Jung C. G. 1968. Archetypes and the collective unconscious. In The Collected Works of C. G. Jung, pages 3-41. Princeton University Press, Princeton, NJ. Translated by R. F. C. Hull, vol. 9, 2nd edition.Google Scholar

  • Jung C. G. Concerning Mandala symbolism [1950]. In The Collected Works of C. G. Jung, pages 355-384. Princeton University Press, Princeton, NJ, 1968. Translated by R. F. C. Hull, vol. 9, 2nd edition.Google Scholar

  • Jung C. G. 1972a. On psychic energy. In The Collected Works of C. G. Jung, pages 3-66. Princeton University Press, Princeton, NJ. Translated by R. F. C. Hull, vol. 8, 2nd Edition.Google Scholar

  • Jung C. G. 1972b. On the nature of the psyche. In The Collected Works of C. G. Jung, pages 159-236. Princeton University Press, Princeton, NJ. Translated by R. F. C. Hull, vol. 8, 2nd Edition.Google Scholar

  • Hutchins E. 2005. Material anchors for conceptual blends. Journal of Pragmatics 37: 1555-1577.Google Scholar

  • Laland K. N., K. Sterelny. 2006. Perspective: Seven reasons (not) to neglect niche construction. Evolution International Journal of Organic Evolution, 60(9):4757-4779.Google Scholar

  • Laland K. N., F. J. Odling-Smee, and M. W. Feldman. 2000. Niche construction, biological evolution and cultural change. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 23(1):131-175.Google Scholar

  • Leyton M. 1999. Symmetry, Causality, Mind. The MIT Press, Cambridge, MA.Google Scholar

  • Leyton M. 2001. A Generative Theory of Shape. Springer, Berlin, 2001.Google Scholar

  • Magnani, L. 2001. Philosophy and Geometry. Theoretical and Historical Issues. Kluwer Academic Publisher, Dordrecht.Google Scholar

  • Magnani L. 2007. Morality in a Technological World. Knowledge as Duty. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.Google Scholar

  • Magnani L. 2009. Abductive Cognition. The Epistemological and Eco-Cognitive Dimensions of Hypothetical Reasoning. Springer, Heidelberg/Berlin.Google Scholar

  • Magnani L. 2011. Understanding Violence. The Intertwining of Morality, Religion, and Violence: A Philosophical Stance. Springer, Heidelberg/Berlin.Google Scholar

  • Magnani L. 2017. The Abductive Structure of Scientific Creativity. An Essay on the Ecology of Cognition. Springer, Cham, Switzerland.Google Scholar

  • Magnani L. ,T. Bertolotti (eds.). 2017. Handbook of Model-Based Science. Springer, Heidelberg/Berlin.Google Scholar

  • Magnani L., S. Arfini, and T. Bertolotti. 2016. Of habit and abduction. Preserving ignorance or attaining knowledge? In: D. West and M. Anderson (eds.), Consensus on Peirce’s Concept of Habit: Before and Beyond Consciousness, pages 361-377. Springer.Google Scholar

  • Menary R. 2010. The Extended Mind, MIT Press, Cambridge, MA.Google Scholar

  • Netz R. 1999. The Shaping of Deduction in Greek Mathematics. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.Google Scholar

  • Park W. 2017. On Lorenzo Magnani’s manipulative abduction. In L. Magnani and T. Bertolotti (eds.), Handbook of Model-Based Science, pages 197 -213. Springer, Heidelberg/Berlin.Google Scholar

  • Polanyi M. 1966. The Tacit Dimension. Routledge & Kegan Paul, London.Google Scholar

  • Sterelny K., B. Fraser. 2016. Evolution and moral realism. The British Journal for the Philosophy of Science, 4(68):981-1006.Google Scholar

  • Sterelny K. 2010. Minds: Extended or scaffolded? Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 9(4):465-481.Google Scholar

  • Zhang J., V. L. Patel. 2006. Distributed cognition, representation, and affordance. Cognition & Pragmatics 14(2): 333-341.Google Scholar

About the article

Received: 2018-01-10

Accepted: 2018-08-20

Published Online: 2018-11-10

Published in Print: 2018-10-01


Citation Information: Open Information Science, Volume 2, Issue 1, Pages 147–155, ISSN (Online) 2451-1781, DOI: https://doi.org/10.1515/opis-2018-0011.

Export Citation

© by Lorenzo Magnani, published by De Gruyter. This work is licensed under the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 License. BY-NC-ND 4.0

Comments (0)

Please log in or register to comment.
Log in