Jump to ContentJump to Main Navigation
Show Summary Details
More options …

Polish Psychological Bulletin

The Journal of Committee for Psychological Sciences of Polish Academy of Sciences

4 Issues per year


CiteScore 2016: 0.33

SCImago Journal Rank (SJR) 2016: 0.185
Source Normalized Impact per Paper (SNIP) 2016: 0.258

Open Access
Online
ISSN
1641-7844
See all formats and pricing
More options …
Volume 45, Issue 4

Issues

You are so kind – and I am kind and smart: Actor – Observer Differences in the Interpretation of On-going Behavior

Andrea E. Abele / Susanne Bruckmüller / Bogdan Wojciszke
Published Online: 2014-12-16 | DOI: https://doi.org/10.2478/ppb-2014-0048

Abstract

: The dual perspective model of agency and communion predicts that observers tend to interpret a target’s behavior more in terms of communion than agency, whereas actors interpret their behavior more in terms of agency. The present research for the first time tests this model in real interactions. Previously unacquainted participants had a short conversation and afterwards rated their own behavior (actor perspective) and their interaction partner’s behavior (observer perspective) in terms of agency(self-confident, assertive) and communion(trustworthy, empathic). Supporting the dual perspective model, observers rated the actor’s behavior higher on communion than on agency, and higher on communion than actors themselves did. Findings for actors were more complex: Actors rated their own behavior as more agentic than observers did. However, they also rated their behavior high on communion. We discuss implications for the dual perspective model as well as for (mis)understandings in social interactions.

Keywords : actor-observer differences; agency; communion; dual perspective model

References

  • Abele, A. E. (2003). The dynamics of masculine-agentic and femininecommunal traits. Findings from a prospective study. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 768-776.Google Scholar

  • Abele, A.E.,& Brack, S. (2013). Preference for other persons’ traits is dependent on the kind of social relationship. Social Psychology, 44, 84-94.Web of ScienceCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Abele, A.E., & Bruckmüller, S. (2011). The bigger one of the “Big Two”: Preferential processing of communal information. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 47, 935-948.Google Scholar

  • Abele, A. E., Rupprecht, T., & Wojciszke, B. (2008). The influence of success and failure experiences on agency. European Journal of Social Psychology, 38, 436-448.Google Scholar

  • Abele, A. E., Uchronski, M., Suitner, C., & Wojciszke, B. (2008). Towards an operationalization of the fundamental dimensions of agency and communion: Trait content ratings in five countries considering valence and frequency of word occurrence. European Journal of Social Psychology, 38, 1202-1217.Web of ScienceGoogle Scholar

  • Abele, A.E.,& Wojciszke, B. (2014). Communal and agentic content in social cognition: A dual perspective model. In Olson, J. & Zanna, M. (Eds), (2014). Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 46, in press.Web of ScienceCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Abele, A. E., & Wojciszke, B. (2007). Agency and communion from the perspective of self-versus others. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93, 751-763.Web of ScienceGoogle Scholar

  • Bakan, D. (1966). The duality of human existence. An essay on psychology and religion. Chicago: Rand Mcnally.Google Scholar

  • Carlston, D. E., & Skowronski, J. J. (1994). Savings in the relearning of trait information as evidence for spontaneous inference generation.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66, 840-856.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Corrigall, E. A., & Konrad, A. M. (2007). Gender role attitudes and careers: A longitudinal study. Sex Roles, 56, 847-855.Google Scholar

  • De Bruin, E. N. M., & Van Lange, P. A. M. (1999). Impression formation and cooperative behavior. European Journal of Social Psychology, 29, 305-328.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • De Bruin, E. N. M., & Van Lange, P. A. M. (2000). What people look for in others: Influences of the perceiver and the perceived on information selection. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26, 206-219.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Fiske, S. T., Cuddy, A. J. C., & Glick, P. (2007). Universal dimensions of social cognition: Warmth and competence. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(2), 77-83.Web of ScienceCrossrefPubMedGoogle Scholar

  • Gebauer, J., Wagner, J., Sedikidis, C., & Neberich, W. (2012). Agencycommunion and self-esteem relations are moderated by culture, religiosity, age, and sex: Evidence for the “self-centrality breeds selfenhancement” principle. Journal of Personality, 81, 261-275.Web of ScienceGoogle Scholar

  • Heider, F. (1958). The psychology of interpersonal relations. New York: John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar

  • Helgeson, V. S. (1994). Relation of agency and communion to well-being: Evidence and potential explanations. Psychological Bulletin, 116, 412-428.Google Scholar

  • Jones, E. E., & Nisbett, R. E. (1971). The actor and the observer: Divergent perceptions of the causes of behavior. Morristown, NJ: General Learning Press.Google Scholar

  • Jones, E. E., & Thibaut, J. W. (1958). Interaction goals as bases of inference in interpersonal perception. In Tagiuri, R.& Petrullo, L. (Eds.), Person perception and interpersonal behavior (pp. 151-178). Palo Alto, CA: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar

  • Judd, C. M., James-Hawkins, L., Yzerbyt, V., & Kashima, Y. (2005).Fundamental dimensions of social judgment: Understanding the relations between judgments of competence and warmth. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89, 899-913.Web of ScienceGoogle Scholar

  • Kenworthy, J., & Tausch, N. (2008). Beliefs about the utility and stability of trait attributions in an intergroup context: Differences between warmth and competence. European Journal of Social Psychology, 38, 1121-1129.Google Scholar

  • Kashy, D. A., & Kenny, D. A. (2000). The analysis of data from dyads and groups. In Reis, H. T.& Judd, C. M. (Eds.), Handbook of research methods in social and personality psychology (pp. 451-477). New York, NY, US: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar

  • Kirchmeyer, C. (1998). Determinants of managerial career success: Evidence and explanation of male/female differences. Journal of Management, 24, 673-692.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Malle, B. (2006). The actor-observer asymmetry in attribution: A (surprising) meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 132(6), 895-919.Google Scholar

  • Malle, B. F., & Knobe, J. (1997). Which behaviors do people explain? A basic actor-observer asymmetry. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72, 288-304.Google Scholar

  • Malle, B. F., Knobe, J. M., & Nelson, S. E. (2007). Actor-observer asymmetries in explanations of behavior: New answers to an old question. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93, 491-514.Google Scholar

  • Newman, L. S., & Uleman, J . S. (1989). Spontaneous trait inference. In Uleman, J. S. &Bargh, J. A. (Eds.), Unintended thought (pp 155-188).New York: Guilford.Google Scholar

  • Nisbett, R. E., Caputo, C., Legant, P., & Marecek, J. (1973). Behavior as seen by the actor and as seen by the observer. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 27, 154-164.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Paulhus, D. L., & Trapnell, P. D. (2008). Self-presentation of personality: An agency-communion framework. In John, O. P., Robins, R. W. & Pervin, L. A. (Eds.), Handbook of personality psychology: Theory and research (3rd ed.) (pp. 492-517). New York, NY, US: Guilford Press.Google Scholar

  • Peeters, G. (1992). Evaluative meanings of adjectives in vitro and in context: Some theoretical implications and practical consequences of positive-negative asymmetry and behavioral-adaptive concepts of evaluation. Psychologica Belgica, 32(2), 211-231.Google Scholar

  • Peeters, G. (2001). In search for a social-behavioral approach-avoidance dimension associated with evaluative trait meanings. Psychologica Belgica, 41(4), 187-203.Google Scholar

  • Peeters, G. (2008). The evaluative face of a descriptive model: Communion and agency in Peabody’s tetradic model of trait organization.European Journal of Social Psychology, 38, 1066-1072.Web of ScienceGoogle Scholar

  • Peeters, G., & Czapinski, J. (1990). Positive-negative asymmetry in evaluations: The distinction between affective and informational negative effects. In Stroebe, W. & Hewstone, M. (Eds.), European review of social psychology (Vol. 1, pp. 33-60). London: Wiley.Google Scholar

  • Rim, S., Uleman, J. S., & Trope, Y. (2009). Spontaneous trait inference and construal level theory: Psychological distance increases nonconscious trait thinking. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 45, 1088-1097.Web of SciencePubMedCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Robins, R. W., Spranca, M. D., & Mendelsohn, G. A. (1996). The actorobserver effect revisited: Effects of individual differences and repeated social interactions on actor and observer attributions.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 375-389.Google Scholar

  • Sande, G. N., Goethals, G. R., & Radloff, C. E. (1988). Perceiving one’s own traits and others’: The multifaceted self. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 13-20.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Scholer, A. A., & Higgins, E. T. (2008). People as resources: Exploring the functionality of warm and cold. European Journal of Social Psychology, 38, 1111-1120.Web of ScienceGoogle Scholar

  • Storms, M. D. (1973). Videotape and the attribution process: Reversing actors’ and observers’ points of view. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Vol. 27, 165-175.PubMedCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Taylor, S. E., & Fiske, S. T. (1975). Point of view and perceptions of causality. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 32, 439-445.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Uchronski, M. (2008). Agency and communion in spontaneous selfdescriptions: Occurrence and situational malleability. European Journal of Social Psychology, 38, 1093-1102.Web of ScienceGoogle Scholar

  • Wojciszke, B. (1994). Multiple meanings of behavior: Construing actions in terms of competence or morality. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 222-232.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wojciszke, B., & Abele, A. E. (2008). The primacy of communion over agency and its reversals in evaluations. European Journal of Social Psychology, 38, 1139-1147.Web of ScienceGoogle Scholar

  • Wojciszke, B., Bazinska, R., & Jaworski, M. (1998). On the dominance of moral categories in impression formation. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 24, 1245-1257.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wojciszke, B., Baryla, W., Parzuchowski, M., Szymkow, A.,& Abele, A.E. (2011). Self-esteem is dominated by agentic over communal information. European Journal of Social Psychology, 41, 617-627.Web of ScienceCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Ybarra, O., Chan, E., & Park, D. (2001). Young and old adults’ concerns about moralityand competence. Motivation and Emotion, 25, 85-100.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Ybarra, O., Chan, E., Park, H., Burnstein, E., Monin, B., & Stanik, C. (2008). Life’s recurring challenges and the fundamental dimensions: An integration and its implications for cultural differences and similarities. European Journal of Social Psychology, 38, 1083-1092.Web of ScienceGoogle Scholar

  • Ybarra, O., Park, H., Stanik, C., & Lee, D. S. (2012). Self-judgment and reputation monitoring as a function of the fundamental dimensions, Temporal perspective, and culture. European Journal of Social Psychology, 42, 200-209. CrossrefWeb of ScienceGoogle Scholar

About the article

Published Online: 2014-12-16

Published in Print: 2014-12-01


Citation Information: Polish Psychological Bulletin, Volume 45, Issue 4, Pages 394–401, ISSN (Online) 1641-7844, DOI: https://doi.org/10.2478/ppb-2014-0048.

Export Citation

© Polish Academy of Sciences, Committee for Psychological Sciences. This work is licensed under the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 3.0 License. BY-NC-ND 3.0

Citing Articles

Here you can find all Crossref-listed publications in which this article is cited. If you would like to receive automatic email messages as soon as this article is cited in other publications, simply activate the “Citation Alert” on the top of this page.

[1]
Andrea E. Abele, Nicole Hauke, Kim Peters, Eva Louvet, Aleksandra Szymkow, and Yanping Duan
Frontiers in Psychology, 2016, Volume 7

Comments (0)

Please log in or register to comment.
Log in