Jump to ContentJump to Main Navigation
Show Summary Details
More options …

Review of Artistic Education

2 Issues per year

Open Access
See all formats and pricing
More options …

Number 13 / Part I. Music. 7. Grounds of Absolute Pitch Development in Yamaha Music School

Dorina Iușcă
Published Online: 2017-03-16 | DOI: https://doi.org/10.1515/rae-2017-0007


Absolute pitch is defined as the ability to identify the pitch class of a certain given sound without the aid of an external reference pitch (Takeuchi & Hulse, 1993; Deutsch, 2002). The incidence of absolute pitch is extremely rare among the general population, respectively 1 in 10.000 people and it depends on testing conditions such as the number of identified sounds, pitch Chroma, pitch height, timbre, register or requested reaction time, and also on subjects musical training commencing and Eastern-Asian origins. The way absolute pitch develops is described by three models: the tone language theory, the early training theory and the genetic theory. The early training theory states that absolute pitch appears due to the beginning of musical lessons during a critical development period situated before the age of 6. The educational implications of this theory are revealed in the principles and activities of Yamaha Music School which employs didactic strategies that naturally develop absolute pitch. Yamaha Music School is the largest private music education system from Japan, established by Torakusu Yamaha in 1954. Up to this day it has extended in 40 countries from Europe, Asia and the American continents, as it has about 710 million students and 30.000 teachers. The present study aims to illustrate a detailed analysis of the way the learning experiences offered by Yamaha School lead to the development of absolute pitch.

Keywords: absolute pitch; Yamaha Music School; early training theory


  • 1. Athos, A., Levinson, B., Kistler, A., Zemansky, J., Bostrom, A., Freimer, N., Gitschier, J. (2007). Dichotomy and perceptual distortions in absolute pitch ability. PNAS, 104(37), 14795-14800.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • 2. Baharloo, S., Johnston, P., Service, S., Gitschier, J., Freimer, N. (1998). Absolute pitch: An approach for identification of genetic and nongenetic components. American Journal of Human Genetics, 62, 224-231.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • 3. Bermudez, P. & Zatorre, R. (2009). A distribution of absolute pitch ability as revealed by computerized testing. Music Perception, 27(2), 89-101.Web of ScienceCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • 4. Brammer, L.M. (1951). Sensory cues in pitch judgement. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 41, 336-340.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • 5. Brown, W., Cammuso, K., Sachs, H., Winklosky, B., Mullane, J., Bernier, R., Svenson, S., Arin, D., Rosen-Sheidley, Folstein, S. (2003). Autism-related language, personality and cognition in people with absolute pitch: Results of a preliminary study. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 33(2), 163-166.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • 6. Chin, C. (2003). The development of absolute pitch: A theory concerning the roles of music training at an early developmental age and individual cognitive style. Psychology of Music, 31(2), 155-171.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • 7. Cohen, A. & Baird, K. (1990). Acquisition of absolute pitch: The question of critical periods. Psychomusicology, 9(1), 31-37.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • 8. Deutsch, D. (2002). The puzzle of absolute pitch. Journal of American Psychological Society, 11(6), 200-204.Google Scholar

  • 9. Deutsch, D., Henthorn, T. Dolson, M. (2004). Absolute pitch, speech and tone language: Some experiments and a proposed framework. Music Perception, 21(3), 339-356.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • 10. Deutsch, D., Henthorn, T., Marvin, E., Xu, H. (2006). Absolute pitch among American and Chinese conservatory students: Prevalence differences and evidence for a speech-related critical period. Journal of Acoustical Society of America, 119(2), 719-722.Google Scholar

  • 11. Deutsch, D., Henthorn, T., Marvin, E., Xu, H. (2006). Absolute pitch among American and Chinese conservatory students: Prevalence differences and evidence for a speech-related critical period. Journal of Acoustical Society of America, 119(2), 719-722.Google Scholar

  • 12. Gervain, J., Vines, B., Chen, L., Seo, R., Hensch, T., Werker, J., Young, A. (2013). Valproate reopens critical-period learning of absolute pitch. Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience, 7(102), 1-11.Google Scholar

  • 13. Gregersen, P., Kowalsky, E., Kohn, N., Marvin, E. (1999). Absolute pitch: Prevalence, ethnic variation and estimation of the genetic component. American Journal of Human Genetics, 65, 911-928.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • 14. Hsieh, I. & Saberi, K. (2009). Virtual pitch extraction from harmonic structures by absolute-pitch musicians. Acoustical Physics, 55(2), 232-239.Web of ScienceCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • 15.Krumhansl, C. (2000). Music perception. Psychological Bulletin, 126(1), 363-366.Google Scholar

  • 16. Levitin, D. & Zatorre, R. (2003). On the nature of early music training and absolute pitch: A reply to Brown, Sachs, Cammuso and Folstein. Music Perception, 21(1), 105-110.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • 17. Lockhead, G. & Byrd, R. (1981). Practically perfect pitch. Journal of Acoustical Society of America, 70(2), 387-389;Google Scholar

  • 18. Miyazachi, K., Makomaska, S., Rakowski, A. (2012). Prevalence of absolute pitch: A comparison between Japanese and Polish music students. Journal of Acoustical Society of America, 132(5), 3484-3493.Google Scholar

  • 19. Miyazaki, K. & Ogawa, Y. (2006). Learning absolute pitch by children: A cross-sectional study. Music Perception, 24(1), 63-78;CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • 20. Miyazaki, K. (1989). Absolute pitch identification: Effects of timbre and pitch region. Music Perception, 7(1), 1-14.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • 21. Parncutt, R. & Levitin, D. (1999). Absolute pitch. Grove Dictionary.Google Scholar

  • 22. Ross, D. & Marks, L. (2009). Absolute pitch in children prior to the beginning of musical training. The Neuroscience and Music III: Disorders and Plasticity: Annals of New York Academy of Science, 1169, 199-204.Google Scholar

  • 23. Schlemmer, K., Kulke, F., Kuchinke, L., Meer, E. (2005). Absolute pitch and pupillary response: Effects of timbre and key color. Psychophysiology, 42, 465-472.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • 24. Sergeant, D.C. (1969). Experimental investigation of absolute pitch. Journal of Research in Music Education, 17, 135-143.Google Scholar

  • 25. Takeuchi, A. & Hulse, S. (1993). Absolute pitch. Psychological Bulletin, 113(2), 345-361.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • 26. Vanzella, P. & Schellenberg E. (2010). Absolute pitch: Effects of timbre on note-naming ability. PLoS ONE, 5(11), e15449.Web of ScienceCrossrefGoogle Scholar

About the article

Published Online: 2017-03-16

Published in Print: 2017-03-01

Citation Information: Review of Artistic Education, Volume 13, Issue 1, Pages 60–65, ISSN (Online) 2501-238X, DOI: https://doi.org/10.1515/rae-2017-0007.

Export Citation

© 2017. This work is licensed under the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 License. BY-NC-ND 4.0

Comments (0)

Please log in or register to comment.
Log in