Jump to ContentJump to Main Navigation
Show Summary Details
More options …

Semiotica

Journal of the International Association for Semiotic Studies / Revue de l'Association Internationale de Sémiotique

Editor-in-Chief: Danesi, Marcel


IMPACT FACTOR 2017: 0.183
5-year IMPACT FACTOR: 0.283

CiteScore 2017: 0.23

SCImago Journal Rank (SJR) 2017: 0.228
Source Normalized Impact per Paper (SNIP) 2017: 0.634

Agenzia Nazionale di Valutazione del Sistema Universitario e della Ricerca: Classe A

Online
ISSN
1613-3692
See all formats and pricing
More options …
Ahead of print

Issues

Semiosis is cognitive niche construction

Pedro Atã / João Queiroz
Published Online: 2019-03-30 | DOI: https://doi.org/10.1515/sem-2018-0092

Abstract

Here we describe Peircean post-1903 semiosis as a processualist conception of meaning, and relate it to contemporary active externalism in Philosophy of Cognitive Science, especially through the notion of cognitive niche construction. In particular, we shall consider the possibility of integrating (a) the understanding of “semiosis as process” within Peirce’s mature semiotics with (b) an elaboration of the concept of cognitive niche from the point of view of niche construction theory and process biology research.

Keywords: semiosis; cognitive niche construction; process philosophy, C. S. Peirce

References

  • Atã, Pedro & João Queiroz. 2013. Icon and abduction: Situatedness in Peircean cognitive semiotics. In Lorenzo Magnani (ed.), Model-based reasoning in science and technology, 301–313. New York: Springer.Google Scholar

  • Atã, Pedro & João Queiroz. 2014. Iconicity in Peircean situated cognitive semiotics. In Torkild Thellefsen & Bent Sorensen (eds.), Charles Sanders Peirce in his own words: 100 years of semiotics, communication and cognition, 527–536. Berlin: Walter de Gruyter.Google Scholar

  • Atã, Pedro & João Queiroz. 2016. Habit in semiosis: Two different perspectives based on hierarchical multi-level system modeling and niche construction theory. In Donna West & Myrdene Anderson (eds.), Consensus on Peirce’s concept of habit: Before and beyond consciousness, 109–119. New York: Springer.Google Scholar

  • Bardone, Emanuele. 2011. Seeking chances: From biased rationality to distributed cognition. New York: Springer.Google Scholar

  • Bergman, Mats. 2009. Peirce’s philosophy of communication. London: Continuum.Google Scholar

  • Bickerton, Derek. 2009. Adam’s tongue. New York: Hill and Wang.Google Scholar

  • Bickhard, Mark. 2007. Modern approaches to language. New Ideas in Psychology 25. 67–69.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bickhard, Mark. 2011. Some consequences (and enablings) of process metaphysics. Axiomathes 21(1). 3–32.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Briscoe, Ted. 1998. Language as a complex adaptive system: Coevolution of language and of the language acquisition device. In Philip Cohen & Wolfgang Wahlster (eds.), Proceedings of the eighth meeting of computational linguistics in the Netherlands conference, 3–40. Nijmegan.Google Scholar

  • Cangelosi, Angelo & Huck Turner. 2002. L’emergere del linguaggio. In A. M. Borghi & T. Iachini (eds.), Scienze della mente, 227–244. Bologna: Il Mulino.Google Scholar

  • Chemero, Anthon. 2009. Radical embodied cognitive science. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar

  • Christiansen, Morten & Simon Kirby. 2003. Language evolution: Consensus and controversies. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 7(7). 300–307.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Clark, Andy. 2004. Natural-born cyborgs: Minds, technologies, and the future of human intelligence. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar

  • Clark, Andy. 2005. Word, niche and super-niche: How language makes minds matter more. Theoria 54. 255–268.Google Scholar

  • Clark, Andy. 2008. Supersizing the mind: Embodiment, action, and cognitive extension. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar

  • Clark, Andy & David Chalmers. 1998. The extended mind. Analysis 58. 7–19.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Davies, Jim & Kourken Michaelian. 2016. Identifying and individuating cognitive systems: A task-based distributed cognition alternative to agent-based extended cognition. Cognitive Processing 17. 307–319.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • de Tienne, Andre. 2003. Learning qua Semiosis. S.E.E.D. Journal 3. 37–53.Google Scholar

  • Emmeche, Claus. 2003. Causal processes, semiosis, and consciousness. In Johanna Seibt (ed.), Process theories: Crossdisciplinary studies in dynamic categories, 313–336. Dordrecht: Kluwer.Google Scholar

  • Fisch, Max H. 1986. Peirce, semeiotic and pragmatism: Essays by Max H. Fisch, Kenneth Laine Ketner & Christian Kloesel (eds.). Indianapolis: Indiana University Press.Google Scholar

  • Gomes, Antonio, Charbel El-Hani, Ricardo Gudwin & João Queiroz. 2007. Towards the emergence of meaning processes in computers from Peircean semiotics. Mind & Society – Cognitive Studies in Economics and Social Sciences 6. 173–187.Google Scholar

  • Hoffmeyer, Jesper. 2008. The semiotic niche. Journal of Mediterranean Ecology 9. 5–30.Google Scholar

  • Hutchins, Edwin. 1995. Cognition in the wild. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar

  • Hutchins, Edwin. 2014. The cultural ecosystem of human cognition. Philosophical Psychology 27(1). 34–49.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hutchins, Edwin & B. Hazlehurst. 1995. How to invent a lexicon: The development of shared symbols in interaction. In Nigel Gilbert & Rosaria Conte (eds.), Artificial societies: The computer simulation of social life, 132–159. London: UCL Press.Google Scholar

  • Kelso, Scott. 1995. Dynamic patterns: The self-organization of brain and behavior. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar

  • Kirsh, David. 2009. Problem solving and situated cognition. In Phillip Robbins & Murat Aydede (eds.), The Cambridge handbook of situated cognition, 264–306. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar

  • Laland, Kevin. 2017. Darwin’s unfinished symphony: How culture made the human mind. Princeton: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar

  • Laland, K. N., J. Odling-Smee & M. W. Feldman. 2000. Niche construction, biological evolution, and cultural change. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 23(1). 131–175.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Laschi, Cecilia & Barbara Mazzolai. 2016. Lessons from animals and plants: The symbiosis of morphological computation and soft robotics. IEEE Robotics & Automation Magazine (September). 107–114.Google Scholar

  • Lewontin, Richard & Richard Levins. 1997. Organism and environment. Capitalism Nature Socialism 8(2). 95–98.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Loula, Angelo, Ricardo Gudwin, Charbel El-Hani & Joao Queiroz. 2010. The emergence of self-organized symbol based communication in artificial creatures. Cognitive Systems Research 11. 131–147.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Loula, Angelo & Joao Queiroz. 2011. Modeling the emergence and evolutionary history of semiotic systems and processes. International Journal of Signs and Semiotic Systems 1. i-iv.Google Scholar

  • Määttänen, Pentti. 2010. Habits as vehicles of cognition. In Mats Bergman, Sami Paavola, Ahti-Veikko Pietarinen & Henrik Rydenfelt (eds.), Ideas in action: Proceedings of the applying Peirce conference, 201–210. Helsinki: Nordic Pragmatism Network.Google Scholar

  • MacLennan, Bruce. 2001. The emergence of communication through synthetic evolution. In Mukesh Patel, Vasant Honavar & Karthik Balakrishnan (eds.), Advances in the evolutionary synthesis of intelligent agents, 65–90. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar

  • Magnani, L. 2009. Abductive cognition: The epistemological and eco-cognitive dimensions of hypothetical reasoning. Berlin: Springer.Google Scholar

  • Magnani, Lorenzo. 2007. Creating chances through cognitive niche construction. In Bruno Apolloni (ed.), Knowledge-based intelligent information and engineering systems, 917–925. Berlin: Springer-Verlag.Google Scholar

  • Menary, Richard (ed.). 2010. The extended mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar

  • Merrell, Floyd. 1997. Peirce, signs, and meaning. Toronto: University of Toronto Press.Google Scholar

  • Nicholson, Daniel & John Dupré. 2018. Everything flows: Towards a processual philosophy of biology. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar

  • Odling-Smee, F. John, Kevin Laland & Marcus Feldman. 2003. Niche construction: The neglected process in evolution. Princeton: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar

  • Peirce, Charles S. 1931–1966. The collected papers of Charles S. Peirce, Vol. 8, C. Hartshorne, P. Weiss & A. W. Burks (eds.). Cambridge: Harvard University Press. [Reference to Peirce’s papers will be designated CP followed by volume and paragraph number.].Google Scholar

  • Peirce, Charles S. 1967. Manuscripts in the Houghton Library of Harvard University, as identified by Richard Robin, Annotated catalogue of the Papers of Charles S. Peirce. Amherst: University of Massachusetts Press. [Reference to Peirce’s manuscripts will be designated MS or L.].Google Scholar

  • Peirce, Charles S. 1982. Writings of Charles S. Peirce, Vol. 6, M. Fisch, E. Moore & C. Kloesel (eds.). Bloomington: Indiana University Press. [Reference to Peirce’s writings will be designated W followed by volume and page number.].Google Scholar

  • Peirce, Charles S. 1992. Essential Peirce: Selected philosophical writings, Vol. 1, (1867–1893), N. Houser & C. Kloesel (eds.). Bloomington: Indiana University Press. [Reference to vol. 1 of Essential Peirce will be designated EP 1.].Google Scholar

  • Peirce, Charles S. 1998. Essential Peirce: Selected philosophical writings, Vol. 2, (1893–1913), Peirce Edition Project (eds.). Bloomington: Indiana University Press. [Reference to vol. 2 of Essential Peirce will be designated EP 2.].Google Scholar

  • Peterson, A. Townsend. 2011. Ecological niches and geographic distributions. Princeton: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar

  • Pinker, Steven. 2003. Language as an adaptation to the cognitive niche. In Morten Christiansen & Simon Kirby (eds.), Language evolution, 16–35. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar

  • Pinker, Steven. 2010. The cognitive niche: Coevolution of intelligence, sociality, and language. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 107. 8993–8999.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Port, Robert. 2009. The dynamics of language. In Robert Meyers (ed.), Encyclopedia of complexity and system science, 2310–2323. Heidelberg: Springer-Verlag.Google Scholar

  • Queiroz, João & Charbel El-Hani. 2006a. Semiosis as an emergent process. Transactions of the Charles S. Peirce Society 42(1). 78–116.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Queiroz, João & Charbel El-Hani. 2006b. Towards a multi-level approach to the emergence of meaning in living systems. Acta Biotheoretica 54. 179–206.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Queiroz, Joao, Claus Emmeche & Charbel El-Hani. 2008. A Peircean approach to information and its relationship with Bateson’s and Jablonka ideas. American Journal of Semiotics 24(1–3). 75–94.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Ransdell, Joseph. 1977. Some leading ideas of Peirce’s semiotic. Semiotica 19(3–4). 157–178.Google Scholar

  • Ransdell, Joseph. 2003. The relevance of Peircean semiotic to computational intelligence augmentation. S.E.E.D. Journal 3(3). 5–36.Google Scholar

  • Rescher, Nicholas. 1996. Process metaphysics: An introduction to process philosophy. New York: SUNY Press.Google Scholar

  • Rosenthal, Sandra. 1994. Charles Peirce’s pragmatic pluralism. Albany, NY: State University of New York Press.Google Scholar

  • Scott-Phillips, Thomas, Kevin Laland, David Shuker, Thomas Dickins & Stuart West. 2013. The niche construction perspective: A critical appraisal. Evolution 68(5). 1231–1243.Google Scholar

  • Seibt, Johanna. 2012. Process philosophy. In Edward N. Zalta (ed.), The Stanford encyclopedia of philosophy. https://plato.stanford.edu/archives/spr2018/entries/process-philosophy/ accessed 25 January 2019).

  • Seifert, Colleen. 1999. Situated cognition and learning. In R. A. Wilson & F. C. Keil (eds.), MIT Encyclopedia of the cognitive sciences, 767–769. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar

  • Sinha, Chris. 2009. Language as a biocultural niche and social institution. In Vyvyan Evans & Stéphanie Pourcel (eds.), New directions in cognitive linguistics, 239–310. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar

  • Skagestad, Peter. 1999. Peirce’s inkstand as an external embodiment of mind. Transactions of the Charles S. Peirce Society 35(3). 551–561.Google Scholar

  • Skagestad, Peter. 2004. Peirce’s semeiotic model of the mind. In Cheryl Misak (ed.), The Cambridge companion to Peirce, 241–256. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar

  • Steels, Luc. 2000. Language as a complex adaptive system. In M. Schoenauer, K. Deb, G. Rudolph, X. Yao, E. Lutton, J. J. Merelo & H.-P. Schwefel (eds.), Proceedings of the sixth international conference on parallel problem solving from nature, 17–26. Dordrecht: Springer.Google Scholar

  • Steels, Luc. 2003. Evolving grounded communication for robots. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 7(7). 308–312.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Tooby, John & Irven DeVore. 1987. The reconstruction of hominid evolution through strategic modeling. In Warren Kinzey (ed.), The evolution of human behavior: Primate models, 183–237. Albany: SUNY Press.Google Scholar

  • Tribble, Evelyn. 2005. Distributing cognition in the globe. Shakespeare Quarterly 56(2). 135–155.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Vogt, Paul. 2002. The physical symbol grounding problem. Cognitive Systems Research 3(3). 429–457.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wagner, Kyle, James A. Reggia, Juan Uriagereka & Gerald S. Wilkinson. 2003. Progress in the simulation of emergent communication and language. Adaptive Behavior 11(1). 37–69.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wheeler, Michael. 2005. Reconstructing the cognitive world – The next step. Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar

About the article

Published Online: 2019-03-30


Citation Information: Semiotica, ISSN (Online) 1613-3692, DOI: https://doi.org/10.1515/sem-2018-0092.

Export Citation

© 2019 Walter de Gruyter GmbH, Berlin/Boston.Get Permission

Comments (0)

Please log in or register to comment.
Log in