Jump to ContentJump to Main Navigation
Show Summary Details
More options …

Scandinavian Journal of Pain

Official Journal of the Scandinavian Association for the Study of Pain

Editor-in-Chief: Breivik, Harald


CiteScore 2018: 0.85

SCImago Journal Rank (SJR) 2018: 0.494
Source Normalized Impact per Paper (SNIP) 2018: 0.427

Online
ISSN
1877-8879
See all formats and pricing
More options …
Volume 15, Issue 1

Issues

How do medical students use and understand pain rating scales?

Pardis Zalmay / Amanda C. de C. Williams
  • Corresponding author
  • Research Dept of Clinical, Educational & Health Psychology, University College London, Gower St, London, WC1E 6BT, UK
  • Email
  • Other articles by this author:
  • De Gruyter OnlineGoogle Scholar
Published Online: 2017-04-01 | DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.sjpain.2016.12.007

Abstract

Background and aims

Pain is a multidimensional experience that is difficult to describe and to assess. To scale current pain, assessment refers to a maximum level of pain, but little is known about this process. Further, clinicians tend to underestimate patients’ pain, with or without patients’ own reports, and to underestimate to a greater extent with more clinical experience, possibly due to recalibration of a personal pain scale with increasing exposure to severe pain. We sought to determine how medical students rated pain in early years of clinical exposure, and in relation to experience of their own and others’ worst pains.

Methods

An online survey sampled medical students’ rating and description of their own worst pain and of that witnessed in another; also what would cause the maximum level of pain and what behaviours characterised it. Last, they indicated their preference among pain scales.

Results

Thirty-six medical students provided responses, the majority in their first six months of clinical exposure. Students’ own worst pain was rated a mean of 6.7/10 (s.d. 1.6) on a numerical scale; causes were diverse but with many bone fractures. Mean worst pain observed in another was rated 8.6/10 (s.d. 1.4); causes included fractures, gallstones, and sickle cell crises. Another’s worst pain was significantly higher (mean 9.4, s.d. 0.8 vs mean 8.0 s.d. 1.4) when rated after the student’s own pain than before it (presentation order randomised).

We found no effect of clinical exposure on estimation of worst pain in another person, nor was there a personal tendency to rate pain using more or less extreme values. Students expected pain of 10/10 to be presented with many verbal, facial and whole body behaviours, and signs of physiological stress. Collectively, behavioural descriptions were rich and varied, but with many incompatibilities: for instance, between ‘writhing’ and ‘rigidity’ expected in the person with extreme pain. Most students preferred the numerical rating scale over visual analogue and verbal scales.

Conclusions

The study requires replication, particularly for clinical experience, where we found no significant difference in estimation of another’s pain over the first three years of medical students’ clinical exposure, but the comparison was underpowered. Despite no systematic individual difference in using pain ratings, there was a marked effect of rating another’s worst pain higher when the rater had previously rated his/her own worst pain. This suggests anchoring estimate of another’s pain in personal pain experience, and a possible way to mitigate clinicians’ underestimation of patients’ pain.

Medical students’ recognition of the importance of facial expression in indicating another’s pain severity was encouraging, but most students anticipated only a narrow range of behaviours associated with extreme pain, thereby excluding other authentic behaviours.

Implications

Many clinical guidelines mandate regular pain assessment for hospital inpatients, and encourage routine assessment in community and outpatient settings, in order to decide on and monitor treatment. Replication and elaboration of this study could extend our understanding of how clinicians interpret pain scales completed by patients, and how they estimate patients’ pain.

This article offers supplementary material which is provided at the end of the article.

Keywords: Pain estimation; Pain behaviour; Pain expression; Numerical rating scale; Worst pain

References

  • [1]

    IASP (International Association for the Study of Pain). Pain terms, a current list with definitions and notes on usage; 1994. http://www.iasp-pain.org/Taxonomy#Pain [accessed 12.10.16].

  • [2]

    Melzack R, Wall PD. Pain mechanisms: a new theory. Science 1965;150:971–9.Google Scholar

  • [3]

    Woolf CJ. What is this thing called pain? J Clin Investigation 2010;120:3742–4.Google Scholar

  • [4]

    Tracey I, Bushnell MC. How neuroimaging studies have challenged us to rethink: is chronic pain a disease? J Pain 2009;10:1113–20.Google Scholar

  • [5]

    Hjermstad MJ, Fayers PM, Haugen DF, Caraceni A, Hanks GW, Loge JH, Fainsinger R, Aass N, Kaasa S. Studies comparing Numerical Rating Scales, Verbal Rating Scales, and Visual Analogue Scales for assessment of pain intensity in adults: a systematic literature review. J Pain Symptom Manage 2011;41:1073–93, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jpainsymman.2010.08.016.

  • [6]

    Litcher-Kelly L, Martino SA, Broderick JE, Stone AA. A systematic review of measures used to assess chronic musculoskeletal pain in clinical and randomized controlled clinical trials. J Pain 2007;8:906–13, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jpain.2007.06.009.

  • [7]

    Jensen MP, Karoly P. Self-report scales and procedures for assessing pain in adults. In: Turk DC, Melzack R, editors. Handbook of pain assessment. 3rd ed. New York: Guilford Press; 2011. p. 9–44.Google Scholar

  • [8]

    Ferreira-Valente MA, Pais-Ribeiro JL, Jensen MP. Validity of four pain intensity rating scales. Pain 2011;152:2399–404, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.pain.2011.07.005.

  • [9]

    Wood BM, Nicholas MK, Blyth F, Asghari A, Gibson S. Assessing pain in older people with persistent pain: the NRS is valid but only provides part of the picture. J Pain 2010;11:1259–65, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jpain.2010.02.025.

  • [10]

    Boonstra AM, Schiphorst Preuper HR, Balk GA, Stewart RE. Cut-off points for mild, moderate, and severe pain in patients with chronic musculoskeletal pain. Pain 2014;155:2545–50, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.pain.2014.09.014.

  • [11]

    Farrar JT, Portenoy RK, Berlin JA, Kinman JL, Strom BL. Defining the clinically important difference in pain outcome measures. Pain 2000;88:287–94.Google Scholar

  • [12]

    Stone AA, Schneider S, Broderick JE, Schwartz JE. Single-day pain assessments as clinical outcomes: not so fast. Clin J Pain 2014;30:739–43.Google Scholar

  • [13]

    Broderick JE, Stone AA, Calvanese P, Schwartz JE, Turk DC. Recalled pain ratings: a complex and poorly defined task. J Pain 2006;7:142–9.Google Scholar

  • [14]

    Williams ACDC, Davies HTO, Chadury Y. Simple pain rating scales hide complex idiosyncratic meanings. Pain 2000;85:457–63, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0304-3959(99)00299-7.

  • [15]

    Hodgins M. Interpreting the meaning of pain severity scores. Pain Res Manage 2002;7:192–8.Google Scholar

  • [16]

    Tait RC, Chibnall JT, Kalauokalani D. Provider judgments of patients in pain: seeking symptom certainty. Pain Med 2009;10:11–34.Google Scholar

  • [17]

    Kappesser J, Williams ACDC. Pain estimation: asking the right questions. Pain 2010;148:184–7, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.pain.2009.10.007.

  • [18]

    Green CR, Anderson KO, Baker TA, Campbell LC, Decker S, Fillingim R, Kaloukalani DA, Lasch KE, Myers C, Tait RC, Todd KH, Vallerand AH. The unequal burden of pain: confronting racial and ethnic disparities in pain. Pain Med 2003;4:277–94.Google Scholar

  • [19]

    Schäfer G, Prkachin KM, Kaseweter KA, Williams ACDC. Health care providers’ judgments in chronic pain: the influence of gender and trustworthiness. Pain 2016;157:1618–25, http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/j.pain.0000000000000536.

  • [20]

    De Ruddere L, Goubert L, Stevens M, Williams ACdeC, Crombez G. Discounting pain in the absence of medical evidence is explained by negative evaluation of the patient. Pain 2013;154:669–76, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.pain.2012.12.018.

  • [21]

    Kappesser J, Williams ACDC, Prkachin KM. Testing two accounts of pain underestimation. Pain 2006;124:109–16, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.pain.2006.04.003.

  • [22]

    Green AD, Tripp DA, Sullivan MJL, Davidson M. The relationship between empathy and estimates of observed pain. Pain Med 2009;10:381–92.Google Scholar

  • [23]

    Riva P, Rusconi P, Montali L, Cherubini P. The influence of anchoring on pain judgment. J Pain Symptom Manage 2011;42:265–76.Google Scholar

  • [24]

    Michael GE, Sporer KA, Youngblood GM. Women are less likely than men to receive prehospital analgesia for isolated extremity injuries. Amer J Emerg Med 2007;25:901–6, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ajem.2007.02.001.

  • [25]

    Staton LJ, Panda M, Chen I, Genao I, Kurz J, Pasanen M, Mechaber AJ, Menon M, O’Rorke J, Wood J, Rosenberg E, Faeslis C, Carey T, Calleson D, Cykert S. When race matters: disagreement in pain perception between patients and their physicians in primary care. J Nat Med Assoc 2007;99:532–8.Google Scholar

  • [26]

    Kim HS, Schwartz-Barcott D, Tracy SM, Fortin JD, Sjöström B. Strategies of pain assessment used by nurses on surgical units. Pain Manage Nurs 2005;6:3–9, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.pmn.2004.12.001.

  • [27]

    Williams ACDC, Craig K. Editorial A science of pain expression? Pain 2006;125:202–3.Google Scholar

  • [28]

    Prkachin KM. Facial pain expression. Pain Manage 2011;1:367–76, http://dx.doi.org/10.2217/pmt.11.22.

  • [29]

    Paul F, Erdfelder E. GPOWER: a priori-, post hoc-, and compromise power analyses for MS-DOS [computer program]. Bonn, Germany: Bonn University; 1992. http://rd.springer.com/article/10.3758/BF03203630 [accessed 31.10.16].

  • [30]

    Qualtrics software. https://www.qualtrics.com/ [accessed 31.10.16].

  • [31]

    Treister R, Nielsen CS, Stubhaug A, Farrar JT, Pud D, Sawilowsky S, Oaklander AL. Experimental comparison of parametric versus nonparametric analyses of data from the cold pressor test. J Pain 2015;16:537–48, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jpain.2015.03.001.

  • [32]

    Braun V, Clarke V. Using thematic analysis in psychology. In: Qualitative research in psychology. Taylor & Francis; 2006. Retrieved from http://eprints.uwe.ac.uk/11735/2/thematic_analysis_revised_-_final.pdf [accessed 31.10.16].

  • [33]

    Helson H. Current trends and issues in adaptation-level theory. Amer Psychologist 1964;19:26–38.Google Scholar

  • [34]

    Prkachin KM, Mass H, Mercer SR. Effects of exposure on perception of pain expression. Pain 2004;111:8–12, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.pain.2004.03.027.

  • [35]

    Hadjistavropoulos T, Breau LM, Craig KD. Assessment of pain in adults and children with limited ability to communicate. In: Turk DC, Melzack R, editors. Handbook of pain assessment. 3rd ed. New York: Guilford Press; 2011. p. 260–80.Google Scholar

  • [36]

    Kunz M, Chatelle C, Lautenbacher S, Rainville P. The relation between catastrophizing and facial responsiveness to pain. Pain 2008;140:127–34, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.pain.2008.07.019.

  • [37]

    Hadjistavropoulos T, Craig KD, Duck S, Cano AM, Goubert L, Jackson P, Mogil J, Rainville P, Sullivan M, Williams ACdeC, Vervoort T, Dever Fitzgerald T. A biopsychosocial formulation of pain communication. Psychol Bull 2011;137:910–39.Google Scholar

  • [38]

    Marco CA, Nagel J, Klink E, Baehren D. Factors associated with self-reported pain scores among ED patients. Amer J Emerg Med 2012;30:331–7.Google Scholar

  • [39]

    Garton L, Haythornthwaite C, Wellman B. Studying online social networks. J Computer-Mediated Commun 2006;3, http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1083-6101.1997.tb00062.x.

  • [40]

    Kappesser J, Williams ACdeC. Pain and negative emotions in the face: judgements by health care professionals. Pain 2002;99:197–206, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0304-3959(02)00101-X.

  • [41]

    Drwecki BB, Moore CF, Ward SE, Prkachin KM. Reducing racial disparities in pain treatment: the role of empathy and perspective-taking. Pain 2011;152:1001–6.Google Scholar

  • [42]

    Backonja M, Farrar JT. Are pain ratings irrelevant? Pain Med 2015;16:1247–50.Google Scholar

About the article

Received: 2016-11-05

Revised: 2016-12-21

Accepted: 2016-12-22

Published Online: 2017-04-01

Published in Print: 2017-04-01


Ethical issues: Please see details at beginning of Methods. Informed consent was required and provided; ethical approval was obtained; the study was registered internally at UCL but not on any external database.

Conflict of interest: Neither of the authors has any conflict of interest to declare.


Citation Information: Scandinavian Journal of Pain, Volume 15, Issue 1, Pages 68–72, ISSN (Online) 1877-8879, ISSN (Print) 1877-8860, DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.sjpain.2016.12.007.

Export Citation

© 2016 Scandinavian Association for the Study of Pain.Get Permission

Supplementary Article Materials

Citing Articles

Here you can find all Crossref-listed publications in which this article is cited. If you would like to receive automatic email messages as soon as this article is cited in other publications, simply activate the “Citation Alert” on the top of this page.

[1]
Marzieh Pahlevan, Mohammad Ali Besharat, Ahmad Borjali, and Hojjatollah Farahani
Annales Médico-psychologiques, revue psychiatrique, 2019
[2]
Agnieszka Neumann-Podczaska, Slawomir Tobis, Lyudmila Yermukhanova, and Katarzyna Wieczorowska-Tobis
Medicina, 2019, Volume 55, Number 5, Page 116
[3]
Anupa Pathak, Saurab Sharma, and Mark P. Jensen
PAIN Reports, 2018, Page 1

Comments (0)

Please log in or register to comment.
Log in