Jump to ContentJump to Main Navigation
Show Summary Details
More options …

Scandinavian Journal of Pain

Official Journal of the Scandinavian Association for the Study of Pain

Editor-in-Chief: Werner, Mads


CiteScore 2018: 0.85

SCImago Journal Rank (SJR) 2018: 0.494
Source Normalized Impact per Paper (SNIP) 2018: 0.427

Online
ISSN
1877-8879
See all formats and pricing
More options …
Volume 16, Issue 1

Issues

The disruptive effects of pain on multitasking in a virtual errands task

David J. Moore
  • Corresponding author
  • Liverpool John Moores University, Liverpool, UK
  • Research Centre for Brain and Behaviour, Liverpool John Moores University, Liverpool, UK
  • Email
  • Other articles by this author:
  • De Gruyter OnlineGoogle Scholar
/ Anna S. Law
  • Corresponding author
  • Liverpool John Moores University, Liverpool, UK
  • Research Centre for Brain and Behaviour, Liverpool John Moores University, Liverpool, UK
  • Other articles by this author:
  • De Gruyter OnlineGoogle Scholar
Published Online: 2017-07-01 | DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.sjpain.2017.02.006

Abstract

Background and aims

Pain is known to have a disruptive effect on cognitive performance, but prior studies have used highly constrained laboratory tasks that lack ecological validity. In everyday life people are required to complete more complex sets of tasks, prioritising task completion and recalling lists of tasks which need to be completed, and these tasks continue to be attempted during episodes or states of pain. The present study therefore examined the impact of thermal induced pain on a simulated errand task.

Methods

Fifty-five healthy adults (36 female) performed the Edinburgh Virtual Errands Task (EVET) either during a painful thermal sensation or with no concurrent pain. Participants also completed the Experience of Cognitive Intrusion of Pain (ECIP) questionnaire to measure their self-reported cognitive impact of pain in general life.

Results

Participants who completed the EVET task in pain and who self-reported high intrusion of pain made significantly more errors than those who reported lower intrusion on the ECIP.

Conclusions

Findings here support the growing literature that suggests that pain has a significant impact on cognitive performance. Furthermore, these findings support the developing literature suggesting that this relationship is complex when considering real world cognition, and that self-report on the ECIP relates well to performance on a task designed to reflect the complexities of everyday living.

Implications

If extrapolated to chronic pain populations, these data suggest that pain during complex multitasking performance may have a significant impact on the number of errors made. For people highly vulnerable to cognitive intrusion by pain, this may result in errors such as selecting the wrong location or item to perform tasks, or forgetting to perform these tasks at the correct time. If these findings are shown to extend to chronic pain populations then occupational support to manage complex task performance, using for example diaries/electronic reminders, may help to improve everyday abilities.

Keywords: Pain; Inteference; Attention; Memory; Cognition; Multitasking

References

  • [1]

    Eccleston C, Crombez G. Pain demands attention: a cognitive–affective model of the interruptive function of pain. Psychol Bull 1999;125:356–66.CrossrefPubMedGoogle Scholar

  • [2]

    Eccleston C. Chronic pain and distraction: an experimental investigation into the role of sustained and shifting attention in the processing of chronic persistent pain. Behav Res Ther 1995;33:391–405.PubMedCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • [3]

    Legrain V, Crombez G, Mauraux A. Controlling attention to nociceptive stimuli with working memory. PLoS ONE 2011;6:e20926.PubMedWeb of ScienceCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • [4]

    Legrain V, Crombez G, Plaghki L, Mouraux A. Shielding cognition from nociception with working memory. Cortex 2013;49:1922–34.Web of ScienceCrossrefPubMedGoogle Scholar

  • [5]

    Buhle J, Wager TD. Performance-dependent inhibition of pain by an executive working memory task. Pain 2010;149:19–26.PubMedWeb of ScienceCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • [6]

    Veldhuijzen DS, Kenemans J, de Bruin C, Olivier B, Volkerts ER. Pain and attention: attentional disruption or distraction? J Pain 2006;7:11–20.CrossrefPubMedGoogle Scholar

  • [7]

    Berryman C, Stanton TR, Bowering KJ, Tabor A, McFarlane A, Moseley GL. Evidence for working memory deficits in chronic pain: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Pain 2013;154:1181–96.CrossrefWeb of SciencePubMedGoogle Scholar

  • [8]

    Berryman C, Stanton TR, Bowering KJ, Tabor A, McFarlane A, Moseley GL. Do people with chronic pain have impaired executive function? A meta-analytical review. Clin Psychol Rev 2014;34:563–79.Web of ScienceCrossrefPubMedGoogle Scholar

  • [9]

    Moriarty O, McGuire BE, Finn DP. The effect of pain on cognitive function: a review of clinical and preclinical research. Progr Neurobiol 2011;93:385–404.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • [10]

    Moore DJ, Keogh E, Eccleston C. The interuptive effect of pain on attention. Q J Exp Psychol 2012;65:565–86.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • [11]

    Moore DJ, Keogh E, Eccleston C. The effect of threat on attentional interruption by pain. Pain 2013;154:82–8.Web of SciencePubMedCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • [12]

    Moore DJ, Keogh E, Eccleston C. Headache impairs attentional performance. Pain 2013;154:1840–5.Web of SciencePubMedCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • [13]

    Hamacher D, Hamacher D, Schega L. A cognitive dual task affects gait variability in patients suffering from chronic low back pain. Exp Brain Res 2014;232:3509–13.CrossrefWeb of SciencePubMedGoogle Scholar

  • [14]

    Shallice T, Burgess PW. Deficits in strategy application following frontal lobe damage in man. Brain 1991;114:727–41.CrossrefPubMedGoogle Scholar

  • [15]

    Alderman N, Burgess PW, Knight C, Henman C. Ecological validity of a simplified version of the multiple errands shopping test. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2003;9:31–44.PubMedGoogle Scholar

  • [16]

    Rand D, Rukan SB-A, Weiss PL, Katz N. Validation of the Virtual MET as an assessment tool for executive functions. Neuropsychol Rehabil 2009;19:583–602.CrossrefPubMedWeb of ScienceGoogle Scholar

  • [17]

    Knight RG, Titov N, Crawford M. The effects of distraction on prospective remembering following traumatic brain injury assessed in a simulated naturalistic environment. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2006;12:8–16.Google Scholar

  • [18]

    Keogh E, Moore DJ, Duggan GB, Payne SJ, Eccleston C. The disruptive effects of pain on complex cognitive performance and executive control. PLoS ONE 2013;8:e83272.PubMedCrossrefWeb of ScienceGoogle Scholar

  • [19]

    Roelofs J, Peters ML, McCracken L, Vlaeyen JW. The pain vigilance and awareness questionnaire (PVAQ): further psychometric evaluation in fibromyalgia and other chronic pain syndromes. Pain 2003;101:299–306.CrossrefPubMedGoogle Scholar

  • [20]

    Attridge N, Crombez G, Van Ryckeghem D, Keogh E, Eccleston C. The experience of cognitive intrusion of pain: scale development and validation. Pain 2015;156:1978–90.PubMedWeb of ScienceCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • [21]

    Logie RH, Trawley S, Law A. Multitasking: multiple, domain-specific cognitive functions in a virtual environment. Mem Cogn 2011;39:1561–74.CrossrefWeb of ScienceGoogle Scholar

  • [22]

    Moore DJ, Eccleston C, Keogh E. Does sex moderate the relationship between anxiety and pain? Psychol Health 2013;28:746–64.Web of SciencePubMedCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • [23]

    Mounce C, Keogh E, Eccleston C. A principal components analysis of negative affect-related constructs relevant to pain: evidence for a three component structure. J Pain 2010;11:710–7.Web of SciencePubMedCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • [24]

    McCracken LM, Zayfert C, Gross RT. The pain anxiety symptoms scale: development and validation of a scale to measure fear of pain. Pain 1992;50:67–73.PubMedCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • [25]

    Coons MJ, Hadjistavropoulos HD, Asmundson GJG. Factor structure and psychometric properties of the Pain Anxiety Symptoms Scale-20 in a community physiotherapy clinic sample. Eur J Pain 2004;8:511–6.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • [26]

    Abrams MP, Carleton RN, Asmundson GJG. An exploration of the psychometric properties of the PASS-20 with a nonclinical sample. J Pain 2007;8:879–86.CrossrefPubMedWeb of ScienceGoogle Scholar

  • [27]

    Taylor S, Zvolensky MJ, Cox BJ, Deacon B, Heimberg RG, Ledley DR, Abramowitz JS, Holaway RM, Sadin B, Stewart SH, Coles M, Eng W, Daly ES, Arrindell WA, Bouvard M, Cardenas SJ. Robust dimensions of anxiety sensitivity: development and initial validation of the Anxiety Sensitivity Index-3. Psychol Assess 2007;19:176–88.PubMedCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • [28]

    McNeil DW, Rainwater AJ. Development of the Fear of Pain Questionnaire-III. J Behav Med 1998;21:389–410.CrossrefPubMedGoogle Scholar

  • [29]

    Sullivan MJL, Bishop SR, Pivik J. The Pain Catastrophizing Scale: development and validation. Psychol Assess 1995;7:524–32.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • [30]

    Lovibond SH, Lovibond PF. Manual for the depression anxiety and stress scales. Sydney: Psychology Foundation; 1995.Google Scholar

  • [31]

    Henry JD, Crawford JR. The short-form version of the Depression Anxiety Stress Scales (DASS-21): construct validity and normative data in a large non-clinical sample. Br J Clin Psychol 2005;44:227–39.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • [32]

    Antony MM, Bieling PJ, Cox BJ, Enns MW, Swinson RP. Psychometric properties of the 42-item and 21-item versions of the Depression Anxiety Stress Scales in clinical groups and a community sample. Washington, DC, ETATS-UNIS: American Psychological Association; 1998.Google Scholar

  • [33]

    Clark-Carter D. Quantitative psychological research: a student’s handbook. Hove, UK: Psychology Press; 2004.Google Scholar

  • [34]

    Stevens J. Applied multivariate statistics in the social sciences. 3rd ed. New Jersey: LEA; 1996.Google Scholar

  • [35]

    Poole HM, Wade K, Fairclough S, Elves S. Gaming in the clinic: effects of game demand on pain experience in adult burns patients. In: International Association for the Study of Pain (IASP) World Congress. 2014.Google Scholar

  • [36]

    Peters ML, Vlaeyen JWS, Kunnen AMW. Is pain-related fear a predictor of somatosensory hypervigilance in chronic low back pain patients? Behav Res Ther 2002;40:85–103.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • [37]

    Van Damme S, Crombez G, Eccleston C. Disengagement from pain: the role of catastrophic thinking about pain. Pain 2004;107:70–6.PubMedCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • [38]

    Crombez G, Eccleston C, Van Damme S, Vlaeyen JWS, Karoly P. The Fear Avoidance Model of chronic pain: the next generation. Clin J Pain 2012;28:475–83.Web of ScienceCrossrefPubMedGoogle Scholar

  • [39]

    Keogh E, Cavill R, Moore DJ, Eccleston C. The effects of menstrual-related pain on attentional interference. Pain 2014;155:821–7.Web of ScienceCrossrefPubMedGoogle Scholar

  • [40]

    Edens JL, Gil KM. Experimental induction of pain: utility in the study of clinical pain. Behav Ther 1995;26:197–216.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • [41]

    Moore DJ, Keogh E, Crombez G, Eccleston C. Methods for studying naturally occurring human pain and their analogues. Pain 2013;154:190–9.CrossrefPubMedWeb of ScienceGoogle Scholar

About the article

Research Centre for Brain and Behaviour, Liverpool John Moores University, Liverpool L3 3AF, UK.


Received: 2016-08-26

Revised: 2017-02-07

Accepted: 2017-02-21

Published Online: 2017-07-01

Published in Print: 2017-07-01


Ethical issues: All participants gave full informed consent to participate in this research and this research received ethical approval from the Liverpool John Moores University Research Ethics Committee (REF: 15/NSP/001).

Conflict of interest: The authors have no conflict of interest in producing this manuscript.


Citation Information: Scandinavian Journal of Pain, Volume 16, Issue 1, Pages 29–35, ISSN (Online) 1877-8879, ISSN (Print) 1877-8860, DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.sjpain.2017.02.006.

Export Citation

© 2017 Scandinavian Association for the Study of Pain.Get Permission

Citing Articles

Here you can find all Crossref-listed publications in which this article is cited. If you would like to receive automatic email messages as soon as this article is cited in other publications, simply activate the “Citation Alert” on the top of this page.

[1]
Rena Gatzounis, Martien G.S. Schrooten, Geert Crombez, and Johan W.S. Vlaeyen
PAIN, 2018, Volume 159, Number 2, Page 351
[2]
R. Gatzounis, M.G.S. Schrooten, G. Crombez, and J.W.S. Vlaeyen
European Journal of Pain, 2017

Comments (0)

Please log in or register to comment.
Log in