Jump to ContentJump to Main Navigation
Show Summary Details
More options …
New at De Gruyter

Scandinavian Journal of Pain

Official Journal of the Scandinavian Association for the Study of Pain

Editor-in-Chief: Breivik, Harald

4 Issues per year


CiteScore 2017: 0.84

SCImago Journal Rank (SJR) 2017: 0.401
Source Normalized Impact per Paper (SNIP) 2017: 0.452

Online
ISSN
1877-8879
See all formats and pricing
More options …
Volume 18, Issue 3

“When I feel the worst pain, I look like shit” – body image concerns in persistent pain

Oliver Sündermann
  • Corresponding author
  • Department of Psychology, National University of Singapore, 02-24, 9 Arts Link, 117570 Singapore, Singapore
  • Email
  • Other articles by this author:
  • De Gruyter OnlineGoogle Scholar
/ Karin Rydberg
  • Department of Law, Psychology and Social Work, Center for Health and Medical Psychology (CHAMP), Örebro University, Örebro, Sweden
  • Other articles by this author:
  • De Gruyter OnlineGoogle Scholar
/ Ludwig Linder
  • Department of Law, Psychology and Social Work, Center for Health and Medical Psychology (CHAMP), Örebro University, Örebro, Sweden
  • Other articles by this author:
  • De Gruyter OnlineGoogle Scholar
/ Steven James Linton
  • Department of Law, Psychology and Social Work, Center for Health and Medical Psychology (CHAMP), Örebro University, Örebro, Sweden
  • Other articles by this author:
  • De Gruyter OnlineGoogle Scholar
Published Online: 2018-04-17 | DOI: https://doi.org/10.1515/sjpain-2017-0163

Abstract

Background and aims

Persistent pain is a pervasive condition that is often associated with a distorted body image. Most research into pain and body image investigated neural or physiological correlates (e.g. phantom limb pain), and much less is known about the psychological experience of body image changes in response to pain such as appearance concerns. The aim was to examine body image concerns in people with persistent pain, in particular appearance concerns and related coping behaviours and appearance-related emotions such as anger and shame.

Methods

Design was cross-sectional and data was collected through in-depth semi-structured interviews with people suffering from persistent musculoskeletal pain (n=7; six females; age=19–56), and analysed with inductive thematic analysis (TA).

Results

Two main themes were identified: “Relationship to the painful body” and “Dissatisfaction with the body”, each containing three subthemes, along with the side-theme “Appearance concerns affected by pain and mood”. All participants reported appearance concerns, predominantly about their weight and related coping behaviours such as avoidance of mirrors, exercising or dieting and pain-induced mood changes that were associated with a negative body image.

Conclusions

People with persistent pain report appearance concerns, often related to pain-induced negative mood changes, and reduced functioning. It remains unclear to what extent attitudes towards the body change over time in accordance with pain. A wider concept of body image is required, including the perception of reduced functioning, related appraisals (e.g. “I look weak and old”) and appearance investment.

Keywords: body image; persistent pain; appearance concerns; qualitative research

References

  • [1]

    Cash TF. Body image: past, present, and future. Body Image 2004;1:1–5.CrossrefPubMedWeb of ScienceGoogle Scholar

  • [2]

    Hrabosky JI, Cash TF, Veale D, Neziroglu F, Soll EA, Garner DM, Strachan-Kinser M, Bakke B, Clauss LJ, Phillips KA. Multidimensional body image comparisons among patients with eating disorders, body dysmorphic disorder, and clinical controls: a multisite study. Body Image 2009;6:155–63.Web of ScienceCrossrefPubMedGoogle Scholar

  • [3]

    Ramachandran VS, Hirstein W. The perception of phantom limbs. The DO Hebb lecture. Brain J Neurol 1998;121:1603–30.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • [4]

    Lewis JS, Kersten P, McCabe CS, McPherson KM, Blake DR. Body perception disturbance: a contribution to pain in complex regional pain syndrome (CRPS). PAIN® 2007;133:111–9.Web of ScienceCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • [5]

    Luomajoki H, Moseley GL. Tactile acuity and lumbopelvic motor control in patients with back pain and healthy controls. Br J Sports Med 2011;45:437–40.PubMedCrossrefWeb of ScienceGoogle Scholar

  • [6]

    Moseley GL. I can’t find it! Distorted body image and tactile dysfunction in patients with chronic back pain. Pain 2008;140:239–43.CrossrefWeb of SciencePubMedGoogle Scholar

  • [7]

    Flor H, Braun C, Elbert T, Birbaumer N. Extensive reorganization of primary somatosensory cortex in chronic back pain patients. Neurosci Lett 1997;224:5–8.CrossrefPubMedGoogle Scholar

  • [8]

    Moseley GL, Flor H. Targeting cortical representations in the treatment of chronic pain: a review. Neurorehabil Neural Repair 2012;26:646–52.CrossrefWeb of SciencePubMedGoogle Scholar

  • [9]

    Osumi M, Imai R, Ueta K, Nobusako S, Morioka S. Negative body image associated with changes in the visual body appearance increases pain perception. PLoS One 2014;9:e107376.Web of SciencePubMedCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • [10]

    Purser MM. Chronic pain and body image (Doctoral dissertation) [Internet]. Unpublished; 2010. Available from: https://digital.library.txstate.edu/handle/10877/4464.

  • [11]

    Sündermann O, Wheatley J, Veale D. ‘If you have good skin, you are god. If you have bad skin, you are a piece of rubbish’. Mastery of shame and anger in treatment-resistant body dysmorphic disorder: a single case study. Cogn Behav Ther 2016;9:1–18.Google Scholar

  • [12]

    Crowe M, Whitehead L, Jo Gagan M, Baxter D, Panckhurst A. Self-management and chronic low back pain: a qualitative study. J Adv Nurs 2010;66:1478–86.CrossrefWeb of SciencePubMedGoogle Scholar

  • [13]

    Mond J, Mitchison D, Latner J, Hay P, Owen C, Rodgers B. Quality of life impairment associated with body dissatisfaction in a general population sample of women. BMC Public Health 2013;13:920.CrossrefWeb of ScienceGoogle Scholar

  • [14]

    Neumark-Sztainer D, Paxton SJ, Hannan PJ, Haines J, Story M. Does body satisfaction matter? Five-year longitudinal associations between body satisfaction and health behaviors in adolescent females and males. J Adolesc Health 2006;39:244–51.CrossrefPubMedGoogle Scholar

  • [15]

    Pruzinsky T. Enhancing quality of life in medical populations: a vision for body image assessment and rehabilitation as standards of care. Body Image 2004;1:71–81.CrossrefPubMedGoogle Scholar

  • [16]

    Proulx K. Experiences of women with bulimia nervosa in a mindfulness-based eating disorder treatment group. Eat Disord 2007;16:52–72.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • [17]

    Veale D, Eshkevari E, Kanakam N, Ellison N, Costa A, Werner T. The appearance anxiety inventory: validation of a process measure in the treatment of body dysmorphic disorder. Behav Cogn Psychother 2014;42:605–16.CrossrefPubMedGoogle Scholar

  • [18]

    Brohede S, Wingren G, Wijma B, Wijma K. Validation of the body dysmorphic disorder questionnaire in a community sample of Swedish women. Psychiatry Res 2013;210:647–52.Web of ScienceCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • [19]

    Sullivan MJ, Bishop SR, Pivik J. The pain catastrophizing scale: development and validation. Psychol Assess 1995;7:524.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • [20]

    Sullivan MJ, Thorn B, Haythornthwaite JA, Keefe F, Martin M, Bradley LA, Lefebvre JC. Theoretical perspectives on the relation between catastrophizing and pain. Clin J Pain 2001;17:52–64.PubMedCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • [21]

    Braun V, Clarke V. Using thematic analysis in psychology. Qual Res Psychol 2006;3:77–101.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • [22]

    Guest G, MacQueen KM, Namey EE. Chapter 1. Introduction to thematic analysis. In: Applied Thematic Analysis. London, UK: SAGE, 2012.Google Scholar

  • [23]

    Wideman TH, Sullivan MJ. Reducing catastrophic thinking associated with pain. Pain 2011;1:249–56.Google Scholar

  • [24]

    Marques L, LeBlanc N, Weingarden H, Greenberg JL, Traeger LN, Keshaviah A, Wilhelma A. Body dysmorphic symptoms: phenomenology and ethnicity. Body Image 2011;8:163–7.PubMedCrossrefWeb of ScienceGoogle Scholar

  • [25]

    Veale D. Advances in a cognitive behavioural model of body dysmorphic disorder. Body Image 2004;1:113–25.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • [26]

    Crombez G, Van Damme S, Eccleston C. Hypervigilance to pain: an experimental and clinical analysis. Pain 2005;116: 4–7.CrossrefPubMedGoogle Scholar

  • [27]

    Bair MJ, Robinson RL, Katon W, Kroenke K. Depression and pain comorbidity: a literature review. Arch Intern Med 2003;163:2433–45.PubMedCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • [28]

    Linton SJ, Bergbom S. Understanding the link between depression and pain. Scand J Pain 2011;2:47–54.PubMedCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • [29]

    Linton SJ, Flink IK, Schrooten MG, Wiksell R. Understanding co-occurring emotion and pain: the role of context sensitivity from a transdiagnostic perspective. J Contemp Psychother 2016;46:129–37.Web of ScienceCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • [30]

    Baker JD, Williamson DA, Sylve C. Body image disturbance, memory bias, and body dysphoria: effects of negative mood induction. Behav Ther 1995;26:747–59.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • [31]

    Cash TF, Santos MT, Williams EF. Coping with body-image threats and challenges: validation of the Body Image Coping Strategies Inventory. J Psychosom Res 2005;58:190–9.CrossrefPubMedGoogle Scholar

  • [32]

    Breivik H, Collett B, Ventafridda V, Cohen R, Gallacher D. Survey of chronic pain in Europe: prevalence, impact on daily life, and treatment. Eur J Pain 2006;10:287–333.CrossrefPubMedGoogle Scholar

  • [33]

    Greenwood KA, Thurston R, Rumble M, Waters SJ, Keefe FJ. Anger and persistent pain: current status and future directions. Pain 2003;103:1–5.CrossrefPubMedGoogle Scholar

  • [34]

    de Souto Barreto P, Ferrandez A-M, Guihard-Costa A-M. Predictors of body satisfaction: differences between older men and women’s perceptions of their body functioning and appearance. J Aging Health 2011;23:505–28.Web of SciencePubMedCrossrefGoogle Scholar

About the article

Received: 2017-11-14

Revised: 2018-03-11

Accepted: 2018-03-18

Published Online: 2018-04-17

Published in Print: 2018-07-26


Authors’ statements

Research funding: The study was supported by a grant from The Swedish Foundation for Humanities and Social Sciences.

Conflict of interest: None.

Informed consent: All participants provided written consent to their participation.

Ethical approval: The study was approved by the Regional Ethics Board, Uppsala (2015/480/1).


Citation Information: Scandinavian Journal of Pain, Volume 18, Issue 3, Pages 379–388, ISSN (Online) 1877-8879, ISSN (Print) 1877-8860, DOI: https://doi.org/10.1515/sjpain-2017-0163.

Export Citation

©2018 Scandinavian Association for the Study of Pain. Published by Walter de Gruyter GmbH, Berlin/Boston. All rights reserved..Get Permission

Citing Articles

Here you can find all Crossref-listed publications in which this article is cited. If you would like to receive automatic email messages as soon as this article is cited in other publications, simply activate the “Citation Alert” on the top of this page.

[1]
Claudia G. Levenig, Michael Kellmann, Jens Kleinert, Johanna Belz, Tobias Hesselmann, and Monika I. Hasenbring
Scandinavian Journal of Pain, 2018, Volume 0, Number 0

Comments (0)

Please log in or register to comment.
Log in