Jump to ContentJump to Main Navigation
Show Summary Details
More options …
New at De Gruyter

Scandinavian Journal of Pain

Official Journal of the Scandinavian Association for the Study of Pain

Editor-in-Chief: Breivik, Harald

4 Issues per year


CiteScore 2017: 0.84

SCImago Journal Rank (SJR) 2017: 0.401
Source Normalized Impact per Paper (SNIP) 2017: 0.452

Online
ISSN
1877-8879
See all formats and pricing
More options …
Volume 18, Issue 3

An experimental investigation of the relationships among race, prayer, and pain

Samantha M. Meints
  • Corresponding author
  • Department of Psychology, Indiana University-Purdue University Indianapolis, Indianapolis, IN, USA
  • Orthopaedic and Arthritis Center for Outcomes Research, Brigham and Women’s Hospital, 60 Fenwood Road, BTM Ste. 5016, Boston, MA 02115, USA, Phone: (857) 307-5405, Fax: (617) 525-7900
  • Email
  • Other articles by this author:
  • De Gruyter OnlineGoogle Scholar
/ Catherine Mosher
  • Department of Psychology, Indiana University-Purdue University Indianapolis, Indianapolis, IN, USA
  • Other articles by this author:
  • De Gruyter OnlineGoogle Scholar
/ Kevin L. Rand
  • Department of Psychology, Indiana University-Purdue University Indianapolis, Indianapolis, IN, USA
  • Other articles by this author:
  • De Gruyter OnlineGoogle Scholar
/ Leslie Ashburn-Nardo
  • Department of Psychology, Indiana University-Purdue University Indianapolis, Indianapolis, IN, USA
  • Other articles by this author:
  • De Gruyter OnlineGoogle Scholar
/ Adam T. Hirsh
  • Department of Psychology, Indiana University-Purdue University Indianapolis, Indianapolis, IN, USA
  • Other articles by this author:
  • De Gruyter OnlineGoogle Scholar
Published Online: 2018-05-03 | DOI: https://doi.org/10.1515/sjpain-2018-0040

Abstract

Background and aims

Compared to White individuals, Black individuals demonstrate a lower pain tolerance. Research suggests that differences in pain coping strategies, such as prayer, may mediate this race difference. However, previous research has been cross-sectional and has not determined whether prayer in and of itself or rather the passive nature of prayer is driving the effects on pain tolerance. The aim of this study was to clarify the relationships among race, prayer (both active and passive), and pain tolerance.

Methods

We randomly assigned 208 pain-free participants (47% Black, 53% White) to one of three groups: active prayer (“God, help me endure the pain”), passive prayer (“God, take the pain away”), or no prayer (“The sky is blue”). Participants first completed a series of questionnaires including the Duke University Religion Index, the Coping Strategies Questionnaire-Revised (CSQ-R), and the Pain Catastrophizing Scale. Participants were then instructed to repeat a specified prayer or distractor coping statement while undergoing a cold pressor task. Cold pain tolerance was measured by the number of seconds that had elapsed while the participant’s hand remained in the cold water bath (maximum 180 s).

Results

Results of independent samples t-tests indicated that Black participants scored higher on the CSQ-R prayer/hoping subscale. However, there were no race differences among other coping strategies, religiosity, or catastrophizing. Results of a 2 (Race: White vs. Black)×3 (Prayer: active vs. passive vs. no prayer) ANCOVA controlling for a general tendency to pray and catastrophizing in response to prayer indicated a main effect of prayer that approached significance (p=0.06). Pairwise comparisons indicated that those in the active prayer condition demonstrated greater pain tolerance than those in the passive (p=0.06) and no prayer (p=0.03) conditions. Those in the passive and no prayer distractor conditions did not significantly differ (p=0.70). There was also a trending main effect of race [p=0.08], with White participants demonstrating greater pain tolerance than Black participants.

Conclusions

Taken together, these results indicate that Black participants demonstrated a lower pain tolerance than White participants, and those in the active prayer condition demonstrated greater tolerance than those in the passive and no prayer conditions. Furthermore, Black participants in the passive prayer group demonstrated the lowest pain tolerance, while White participants in the active prayer group exhibited the greatest tolerance. Results of this study suggest that passive prayer, like other passive coping strategies, may be related to lower pain tolerance and thus poorer pain outcomes, perhaps especially for Black individuals. On the other hand, results suggest active prayer is associated with greater pain tolerance, especially for White individuals.

Implications

These results suggest that understanding the influence of prayer on pain may require differentiation between active versus passive prayer strategies. Like other active coping strategies for pain, active prayer may facilitate self-management of pain and thus enhance pain outcomes independent of race. Psychosocial interventions may help religiously-oriented individuals, regardless of race, cultivate a more active style of prayer to improve their quality of life.

Keywords: pain; prayer; race; ethnicity; QST; coping

References

  • [1]

    Faucett J, Gordon N, Levine J. Differences in postoperative pain severity among four ethnic groups. J Pain Symptom Manage 1994;9:383–9.PubMedCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • [2]

    Breitbart W, Rosenfeld B, Passik SD, McDonald MV, Thaler H, Portenoy RK. Pain in ambulatory AIDS patients. I: Pain characteristics and medical correlates. Pain 1996;68:315–21.CrossrefPubMedGoogle Scholar

  • [3]

    Sherwood M, Garcia-Siekavizza A, Meltzer M, Hebert A, Burns A, McGorray S. Glaucoma’s impact on quality of life and its relation to clinical indicators: a pilot study. Ophthalmology 1998;105:561–6.PubMedCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • [4]

    White S, Asher MA, Lai SM, Burton DC. Patients’ perceptions of overall function, pain, and appearance after primary posterior instrumentation and fusion for idiopathic scoliosis. Spine (Phila Pa 1976) 1999;24:1693-9.CrossrefPubMedGoogle Scholar

  • [5]

    Creamer P, Lethbridge-Cejku M, Hochberg MC. Determinants of pain severity in knee osteoarthritis: effect of demographic and psychosocial variables using 3 pain measures. J Rheumatol 1999;26:1785–92.PubMedGoogle Scholar

  • [6]

    Selim A, Fincke G, Ren X, Deyo R, Lee A, Skinner K, Kazis L. Racial differences in the use of lumbar spine radiographs: results from the veterans health study. Spine (Phila Pa 1976) 2001;26:1364–9.CrossrefPubMedGoogle Scholar

  • [7]

    Chapman WP, Jones CM. Variations in cutaneous and visceral pain sensitivity in normal subjects. J Clin Invest 1944;23:81–91.PubMedCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • [8]

    Woodrow K, Friedman GD, Siegelaub AB, Collen M. Pain tolerance: differences according to age, sex and race. Psychosom Med 1972;34:548–56.CrossrefPubMedGoogle Scholar

  • [9]

    Walsh NE, Schoenfeld L, Ramamurthy S, Hoffman J. Normative model for cold pressor test. Am J Phys Med Rehabil 1989;68:6–11.CrossrefPubMedGoogle Scholar

  • [10]

    Fillingim R, Edwards RR, Powell T. The relationship of sex and clinical pain to experimental pain responses. Pain 1999;83:419–25.CrossrefPubMedGoogle Scholar

  • [11]

    Sheffield D, Biles PL, Orom H, Maixner W, Sheps DS. Race and sex differences in cutaneous pain perception. Psychosom Med 2000;62:517–23.CrossrefPubMedGoogle Scholar

  • [12]

    Edwards C, Fillingim RB, Keefe F. Race, ethnicity and pain. Pain 2001;94:133–7.CrossrefPubMedGoogle Scholar

  • [13]

    Campbell C, Edwards R, Fillingim R. Ethnic differences in responses to multiple experimental pain stimuli. Pain 2005;113:20–6.PubMedCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • [14]

    Rahim-Williams FB, Riley JL, Herrera D, Campbell CM, Hastie BA, Fillingim RB. Ethnic identity predicts experimental pain sensitivity in African Americans and Hispanics. Pain 2007;129:177–84.CrossrefPubMedGoogle Scholar

  • [15]

    Meints SM, Hirsh AT. In vivo praying and catastrophizing mediate the race differences in experimental pain sensitivity. J Pain 2015;16:491–7.PubMedCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • [16]

    Turner J, Chapman C. Psychological interventions for chronic pain: a critical review. II Operant conditioning, hypnosis, and cognitive-behavioral therapy. Pain 1982;12:23–46.PubMedCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • [17]

    Keefe F, Crisson J, Urban B, Williams D. Analyzing chronic low back pain: the relative contribution of pain coping strategies. Pain 1990;40:293–301.CrossrefPubMedGoogle Scholar

  • [18]

    Jensen M, Karoly P. Control beliefs, coping efforts, and adjustment to chronic pain. J Consult Clin Psychol 1991;59:431–8.CrossrefPubMedGoogle Scholar

  • [19]

    Jensen MP, Turner J, Romano J. Self-efficacy and outcome expectancies: relationship to chronic pain coping strategies and adjustment. Pain 1991;44:263–9.PubMedCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • [20]

    Rosenstiel AK, Keefe FJ. The use of coping strategies in chronic low back pain patients: Relationship to patient characteristics and current adjustment. Pain 1983;17:33–44.CrossrefPubMedGoogle Scholar

  • [21]

    Jordan M, Lumley M, Leisen C. The relationships of cognitive coping and pain control beliefs to pain and adjustment among African-American and Caucasian women with rheumatoid arthritis. Arthritis Rheumatol 1998;11:80–8.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • [22]

    Edwards RR, Campbell CM, Fillingim RB. Catastrophizing and experimental pain sensitivity: only in vivo reports of catastrophic cognitions correlate with pain responses. J Pain 2005;6:338–9.PubMedCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • [23]

    Andersson G. Chronic pain and praying to a higher power: useful or useless? J Relig Health 2008;47:176–87.CrossrefPubMedGoogle Scholar

  • [24]

    Tuttle DH, Shutty MS, DeGood DE. Empirical dimensions of coping in chronic pain patients: a factorial analysis. Rehabil Psychol 1991;36:179–88.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • [25]

    Rippentrop EA, Altmaier EM, Chen JJ, Found EM, Keffala VJ. The relationship between religion/spirituality and physical health, mental health, and pain in a chronic pain population. Pain 2005;116:311–21.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • [26]

    Tajadini H, Zangiabadi N, Divsalar K, Safizadeh H, Esmaili Z, Rafiei H. Effect of prayer on intensity of migraine headache. J Evid Based Complementary Altern Med 2017;22:37–40.CrossrefPubMedGoogle Scholar

  • [27]

    Elmholdt Jegindø EM, Vase L, Skewes JC, Terkelsen AJ, Hansen J, Geertz AW, Roepstorff A, Jensen TS. Expectations contribute to reduced pain levels during prayer in highly religious participants. J Behav Med 2013;36:413–26.PubMedCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • [28]

    Elmholdt Jegindo E-M, Skewes J, Dietz M, Møller A, Jensen MS, Roepstorff A, Wiech K, Jensen TS, Okamoto H, Rainville P. Reduced pain sensation and reduced BOLD signal in parietofrontal networks during religious prayer. Front Hum Neurosci 2017;11:337.PubMedCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • [29]

    Meints SM, Miller MM, Hirsh AT. Differences in pain coping between Black and White Americans: a meta-analysis. J Pain 2016;17:642–53.PubMedCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • [30]

    Dozois DJ, Dobson KS, Wong M, Hughes D, Long A. Predictive utility of the CSQ in low back pain: individual vs. composite measures. Pain 1996;66:171–80.CrossrefPubMedGoogle Scholar

  • [31]

    Edwards R, Doleys D, Fillingim R. Ethnic differences in pain tolerance: clinical implications in a chronic pain population. Psychosom Med 2001;63:316–23.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • [32]

    Jones A, Kwoh CK, Groeneveld PW, Mor M, Geng M, Ibrahim SA. Investigating racial differences in coping with chronic. J Cross Cult Gerontol 2011;23:339–47.Google Scholar

  • [33]

    Banziger S, van Uden M, Janssen J. Praying and coping : the relation between varieties of praying and religious coping styles. Ment Heal Relig Cult 2008;11:101–18.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • [34]

    Ashby J, Lenhart RS. Prayer as a coping strategy for chronic pain patients. Rehabil Psychol 1994;39:205–9.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • [35]

    Keefe F, Dolan E. Pain behavior and pain coping strategies in low back pain and myofascial pain dysfunction syndrome patients. Pain 1986;24:49–56.CrossrefPubMedGoogle Scholar

  • [36]

    Brown GK, Nicassio PM. Development of a questionnaire for the assessment of active and passive coping strategies in chronic pain patients. Pain 1987;31:53–64.CrossrefPubMedGoogle Scholar

  • [37]

    Parker J, Smarr KL, Buescher KL, Phillips LR, Frank RG, Beck NC, Anderson SK, Walker SE. Pain control and rational thinking. implications for rheumatoid arthritis. Arthritis Rheumatol 1989;32:984–90.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • [38]

    Beckham J, Keefe FJ, Caldwell DS, Roodman AA. Pain coping strategies in rheumatoid arthritis: relationships to pain, disability, depression and daily hassles. Behav Ther 1991;22:113–24.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • [39]

    Vlaeyen J, Linton SJ. Fear-avoidance and its consequences in chronic musculoskeletal pain: a state of the art. Pain 2000;85:317–32.PubMedCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • [40]

    Mercado A, Carroll LJ, Cassidy JD, Côté P. Passive coping is a risk factor for disabling neck or low back pain. Pain 2005;117:51–7.CrossrefPubMedGoogle Scholar

  • [41]

    Riley J, Robinson ME. CSQ: five factors or fiction? Clin J Pain 1997;13:156–62.CrossrefPubMedGoogle Scholar

  • [42]

    Hastie B, Riley JL, Fillingim RB. Ethnic differences in pain coping: factor structure of the coping strategies questionnaire and coping strategies questionnaire-revised. J Pain 2004;5:304–16.CrossrefPubMedGoogle Scholar

  • [43]

    Sullivan M, Bishop S, Pivik J. The pain catastrophizing scale: development and validation. Psychol Assess 1995;7:524–32.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • [44]

    Osman A, Barrios FX, Gutierrez PM, Kopper BA, Merrifield T, Grittmann L. The Pain Catastrophizing Scale: further psychometric evaluation with adult samples. J Behav Med 2000;23:351–65.CrossrefPubMedGoogle Scholar

  • [45]

    Osman A, Barrios FX, Kopper BA, Hauptmann W, Jones J, O’Neill E. Factor structure, reliability, and validity of the Pain Catastrophizing Scale. J Behav Med 1997;20:589–605.PubMedCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • [46]

    D’Eon JL, Harris CA, Ellis JA. Testing factorial validity and gender invariance of the Pain Catastrophizing Scale. J Behav Med 2004;27:361–72.CrossrefPubMedGoogle Scholar

  • [47]

    Van Damme S, Crombez G, Bijttebier P, Goubert L, Van Houdenhove B. A confirmatory factor analysis of the Pain Catastrophizing Scale: invariant factor structure across clinical and non-clinical populations. Pain 2002;96:319–24.CrossrefPubMedGoogle Scholar

  • [48]

    Koenig H, Büssing A. The Duke University Religion Index (DUREL): a five-item measure for use in epidemological studies. Religions 2010;1:78–85.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • [49]

    Gelfand S. The relationship of experimental pain tolerance to pain threshold. Can J Psychol 1964;18:36–42.CrossrefPubMedGoogle Scholar

  • [50]

    Wolff B. Methods of testing pain mechanisms in normal man. In: Wall PD, Melzack R, editors. Textbook of Pain. London: Churchill Livingstone, 1984:186–94.Google Scholar

  • [51]

    Rainville P, Feine JS, Bushnell MC, Duncan GH. A psychophysical comparison of sensory and affective responses to four modalities of experimental pain. Somatosens Mot Res 1992;9:265–77.CrossrefPubMedGoogle Scholar

  • [52]

    Edens JL, Gil KM. Experimental induction of pain: utility in the study of clinical pain. Behav Ther 1995;26:197–216.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • [53]

    Pargament K, Koenig HG, Perez LM. The many methods of religious coping: development and initial validation of the RCOPE. J Clin Psychol 2000;56:519–43.PubMedCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • [54]

    Bialosky JE, Hirsh A, Robinson M, George S. Manipulation of pain catastrophizing: an experimental study of healthy participants. J Pain Res 2008;1:35–41.PubMedGoogle Scholar

  • [55]

    Edwards RR, Moric M, Husfeldt B, Buvanendran A, Ivankovich O. Ethnic similarities and differences in the chronic pain experience: a comparison of African American, Hispanic, and white patients. Pain Med 2005;6:88–98.CrossrefPubMedGoogle Scholar

  • [56]

    Meints SM, Stout M, Abplanalp S, Hirsh AT. Pain-related rumination, but not magnification or helplessness, mediates race and sex differences in experimental pain. J Pain 2017;18:332–9.PubMedCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • [57]

    Brown G, Nicassio PM, Wallston KA. Pain coping strategies and depression in rheumatoid arthritis. J Consult Clin Psychol 1989;57:652–7.CrossrefPubMedGoogle Scholar

  • [58]

    Bade M, Cook SW. Functions of christian prayer in the coping process. J Sci Study Relig 2008;47:123–33.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • [59]

    Melzack R, Dennis S. Neurophysiological foundations of pain. In: Sternbach RA, editor. The Psychology of Pain. New York: Raven Press, 1978:1–28.Google Scholar

  • [60]

    Geisser M, Robinson ME, Riley JL. Pain beliefs, coping, and adjustment to chronic pain. Let’s focus more on the negative. Pain Forum 1999;8:161–8.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • [61]

    McCaffrey AM, Eisenberg DM, Legedza ATR, Davis RB, Phillips RS. Prayer for health concerns: results of a national survey on prevalence and patterns of use. Arch Intern Med 2004;164:858–62.PubMedCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • [62]

    Dezutter J, Wachholtz A, Corveleyn J. Prayer and pain: the mediating role of positive re-appraisal. J Behav Med 2011;34:542–9.PubMedCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • [63]

    Lorig K, Mazonson PD, Holman HR. Evidence suggesting that health education for self-management in patients with chronic arthritis has sustained health benefits while reducing health care costs. Arthritis Rheumatol 1993;36:439–46.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • [64]

    Von Korff M, Barlow W, Cherkin D, Deyo RA. Effects of practice style in managing back pain. Ann Intern Med 1994;121:187–95.PubMedCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • [65]

    Cedraschi C, Desmeules J, Rapiti E, Baumgartner E, Cohen P, Finckh A, Allaz A, Vischer TL. Fibromyalgia: a randomised, controlled trial of a treatment programme based on self management. Ann Rheum Dis 2004;63:290–6.CrossrefPubMedGoogle Scholar

  • [66]

    Morley S, Eccleston C, Williams A. Systematic review and meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials of cognitive behaviour therapy and behaviour therapy for chronic pain in adults, excluding. Pain 1999;58:1299–310.Google Scholar

  • [67]

    Shapiro D. Examining the content and context of meditation: a challenge for psychology in the areas of stress management, psychotherapy, and religion/values. J Humanist Psychol 1994;34:101–35.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • [68]

    Hilton L, Hempel S, Ewing BA, Apaydin E, Xenakis L, Newberry S, Colaiaco B, Maher AR, Shanman RM, Sobero ME, Maglione MA. Mindfulness meditation for chronic pain: systematic review and meta-analysis. Ann Behav Med 2017;51:199–213.PubMedCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • [69]

    Veehof MM, Trompetter HR, Bohlmeijer ET, Schreurs KMG. Acceptance- and mindfulness-based interventions for the treatment of chronic pain: a meta-analytic review. Cogn Behav Ther 2016;45:5–31.PubMedCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • [70]

    Tan G, Jensen MP, Thornby J, Anderson KO. Ethnicity, control appraisal, coping, and adjustment to chronic pain among Black and White Americans. Pain Med 2005;6:18–28.CrossrefPubMedGoogle Scholar

  • [71]

    Chatters L, Taylor R, Jackson J. Religious coping among African Americans, Caribbean Blacks and Non-Hispanic Whites. J Community Psychol 2008;36:371–86.CrossrefPubMedGoogle Scholar

  • [72]

    Lugo L, Stencel S, Green J, Smith G, Cox D, Pong A. U.S. Religious Landscape Survey: Religious Affiliation. US Religious Landscape Survey. Washington, D.C., 2008.Google Scholar

About the article

Received: 2018-02-16

Revised: 2018-03-27

Accepted: 2018-03-30

Published Online: 2018-05-03

Published in Print: 2018-07-26


Authors’ statements

Research funding: This work was funded by a graduate student research award from the Department of Psychology at Indiana University) Purdue University Indianapolis and the National Institutes of health under award number T32 AR055885.

Conflict of interest: The authors declare that there is no conflict of interest regarding the publication of this article.

Informed consent: All participants provided written informed consent prior to participation in the research study.

Ethical approval: This study was approved by the Institutional Review Board at Indiana University-Purdue University Indianapolis.


Citation Information: Scandinavian Journal of Pain, Volume 18, Issue 3, Pages 545–553, ISSN (Online) 1877-8879, ISSN (Print) 1877-8860, DOI: https://doi.org/10.1515/sjpain-2018-0040.

Export Citation

©2018 Scandinavian Association for the Study of Pain. Published by Walter de Gruyter GmbH, Berlin/Boston. All rights reserved..Get Permission

Comments (0)

Please log in or register to comment.
Log in