Jump to ContentJump to Main Navigation
Show Summary Details
More options …

Theoretical Linguistics

An Open Peer Review Journal

Editor-in-Chief: Krifka, Manfred

Ed. by Gärtner, Hans-Martin

4 Issues per year


IMPACT FACTOR 2016: 0.864
5-year IMPACT FACTOR: 1.744

CiteScore 2016: 0.72

SCImago Journal Rank (SJR) 2016: 0.555
Source Normalized Impact per Paper (SNIP) 2016: 1.105

Online
ISSN
1613-4060
See all formats and pricing
More options …
Volume 42, Issue 1-2 (Jul 2016)

Issues

Why formal semantics and primate communication make strange bedfellows

W. Tecumseh Fitch
  • Corresponding author
  • Department of Cognitive Biology, University of Vienna, UZA 1, Biologiezentrum, Althanstrasse 14, 1090 Vienna, Austria
  • Email
  • Other articles by this author:
  • De Gruyter OnlineGoogle Scholar
Published Online: 2016-07-05 | DOI: https://doi.org/10.1515/tl-2016-0003

References

  • Adret, Patrice. 1992. Vocal learning induced with operant techniques: An overview. Netherlands Journal of Zoology 43. 125–142.Google Scholar

  • Aitken, Peter G. & William A. Wilson Jr. 1979. Discriminative vocal conditioning in Rhesus monkeys: Evidence for volitional control? Brain and Language 8. 227–240.Google Scholar

  • Bar-On, Dorit. 2013. Origins of meaning: Must we ‘Go Gricean’? Mind and Language 28. 342–375.Google Scholar

  • Beckers, Gabriël J.L., Johan J. Bolhuis, Kazuo Okanoya & Robert C. Berwick. 2012. Birdsong neurolinguistics: Songbird context-free grammar claim is premature. NeuroReport 23. 139–145.Google Scholar

  • Berntson, Gary G., Sarah T. Boysen, Harold R. Bauer & Michael S. Torello. 1990. Conspecific screams and laughter: Cardiac and behavioral reactions of infant chimpanzees. Developmental Psychobiology 22. 771–787.Google Scholar

  • Bugnyar, Thomas, Stephan A. Reber & Cameron Buckner. 2016. Ravens attribute visual access to unseen competitors. Nature Communications 7. 10506.Google Scholar

  • Burnstein, David D. & Peter C. Wolff. 1967. Vocal conditioning in the guinea pig. Psychonomic Science 8. 39–40.Google Scholar

  • Cheney, Dorothy L. & Robert M. Seyfarth. 1980. Vocal recognition in free-ranging vervet monkeys. Animal Behaviour 28. 362–367.Google Scholar

  • Cheney, Dorothy L. & Robert M. Seyfarth. 1992a. Meaning, reference, and intentionality in the natural vocalizations of monkeys. In T. Nishida, et al. (eds.), Topics in primatology, Volume 1, human origins, 315–330. Tokyo: Tokyo University Press.Google Scholar

  • Cheney, Dorothy L. & Robert M. Seyfarth. 1992b. Precis of ‘How monkeys see the world’. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15. 135–182.Google Scholar

  • Cheney, Dorothy L. & Robert M. Seyfarth. 1998. Why monkeys don’t have language. In G. Petersen (eds.), The Tanner lectures on human values. Salt Lake City: University of Utah Press.Google Scholar

  • Cheney, Dorothy L. & Robert M. Seyfarth. 2007. Baboon metaphysics: The evolution of a social mind. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar

  • Crockford, Catherine, Roman M. Wittig, Roger Mundry & Klaus Zuberbühler. 2012. Wild chimpanzees inform ignorant group members of danger. Current Biology 22. 1–5.Google Scholar

  • Darwin, Charles. 1872. The expression of the emotions in man and animals. London: John Murray.Google Scholar

  • Davila Ross, Michael J. Marina Owren & Elke Zimmerman. 2009. Reconstructing the evolution of laughter in great apes and humans. Current Biology 19. 1106–1111.Google Scholar

  • Eibl-Eibesfeldt, Irenäus. 1973. The expressive behaviour of the deaf-and blind-born. In M. Von Cranach & J. Vine (eds.), Social communication and movement, 163–194. London: Academic.Google Scholar

  • Fischer, Julia, Brandon C. Wheeler & James P. Higham. 2015. Is there any evidence for vocal learning in chimpanzee food calls? Current Biology 25. 1028–1029.Google Scholar

  • Fitch, W. Tecumseh. 1997. Vocal tract length and formant frequency dispersion correlate with body size in rhesus macaques. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 102. 1213–1222.Google Scholar

  • Fitch, W. Tecumseh. 2006. The biology and evolution of music: A comparative perspective. Cognition 100. 173–215.Google Scholar

  • Fitch, W. Tecumseh. 2009. Prolegomena to a future science of biolinguistics. Biolinguistics 3. 283–320.Google Scholar

  • Fitch, W. Tecumseh. 2014. Toward a computational framework for cognitive biology: Unifying approaches from cognitive neuroscience and comparative cognition. Physics of Life Reviews 11. 329–364.Google Scholar

  • Fitch, W. Tecumseh. 2015. Book review: Evolving pragmatics. Current Biology 25. R1110–R1112.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Fitch, W. Tecumseh. & Angela D. Friederici. 2012. Artificial grammar learning meets formal language theory: An overview. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B 367. 1933–1955.Google Scholar

  • Fitch, W. Tecumseh. & Marc D. Hauser. 2004. Computational constraints on syntactic processing in a nonhuman primate. Science 303. 377–380.Google Scholar

  • Fitch, W. Tecumseh, Jürgen Neubauer & Hanspeter Herzel. 2002. Calls out of chaos: The adaptive significance of nonlinear phenomena in mammalian vocal production. Animal Behaviour 63. 407–418.Google Scholar

  • Fitch, W. Tecumseh & Klaus Zuberbühler. 2013. Primate precursors to human language: Beyond discontinuity. In E. Zimmerman et al. (eds.), The evolution of emotional communication, 26–48. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar

  • Grice, H. P. 1957. Meaning. Philosophical Review, 66(3), 377–388.

  • Hailman, Jack P. & Millicent S. Ficken. 1987. Combinatorial animal communication with computable syntax: Chick-a-dee calling qualifies as ‘language’ by structural linguistics. Animal Behavior 34. 1899–1901.Google Scholar

  • Hare, Brian, Josep Call & Michael Tomasello. 2001. Do chimpanzees know what conspecifics know? Animal Behaviour 61. 139–151.Google Scholar

  • Heinz, Jeffrey & William Idsardi. 2013. What complexity differences reveal about domains in language. Topics in Cognitive Science 5. 111–131.Google Scholar

  • Heyes, Celia M. 1998. Theory of mind in nonhuman primates. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 21. 101–134.Google Scholar

  • Hoeschele, Marisa & W. Tecumseh Fitch. 2016. Phonological perception by birds: budgerigars can perceive lexical stress. Animal Cognition 19. 643–654.Google Scholar

  • Jackendoff, Ray. 2002. Foundations of language. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar

  • Jäger, Gerhard & James Rogers. 2012. Formal language theory: refining the Chomsky Hierarchy. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B 267. 1956–1970.Google Scholar

  • Janik, Vincent M. & Peter J.B. Slater. 1998. Context-specific use suggests that bottlenose dolphin signature whistles are cohesion calls. Animal Behaviour 56. 829–838.Google Scholar

  • Katahira, Kentaro, Kenta Suzuki, Kazuo Okanoya & Masato Okada. 2011. Complex sequencing rules of birdsong can be explained by simple hidden Markov processes. PLoS One 6. e24516.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Lal, Harbans. 1967. Operant control of vocal responding in rats. Psychonomic Science 8. 35–36.Google Scholar

  • Lane, Harlan. 1960. Control of vocal responding in chickens. Science 132. 37–38.Google Scholar

  • Larson, Charles R., Dwight Sutton, Eugene M. Taylor & Roger Lindeman. 1973. Sound spectral properties of conditioned vocalizations in monkeys. Phonetica 27. 100–112.Google Scholar

  • Molliver, Mark E. 1963. Operant control of vocal behavior in the cat. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 6. 197–202.Google Scholar

  • Moore, Richard. 2016. Meaning and ostension in great ape gestural communication. Animal Cognition, 19(1), 223–231.Google Scholar

  • Myers, Shirley A., James A. Horel & Henry S. Pennypacker. 1965. Operant control of vocal behavior in the monkey Cebus albifrons. Psychonomic Science 3. 389–390.Google Scholar

  • Owren, Michael J., Jacquelyn A. Dieter, Robert M. Seyfarth & Dorothy L. Cheney. (1993). Vocalizations of rhesus (Macaca mulatta) and Japanese (M. fuscata) macaques cross-fostered between species show evidence of only limited modification. Developmental Psychobiology 26. 389–406.Google Scholar

  • Patel, Aniruddh D. 2008. Music, language, and the brain. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar

  • Penn, Derek C. & Daniel J. Povinelli. 2007. On the lack of evidence that non-human animals possess anything remotely resembling a ‘theory of mind’. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B 362. 731–744.Google Scholar

  • Peretz, Isabelle, Dominique Vuvan, Marie-Élaine Lagrois & Jorge L. Armony. 2015. Neural overlap in processing music and speech. Philosophical Transactions of The Royal Society B 370.Google Scholar

  • Portner, Paul H. 2005. What is meaning: Fundamentals of formal semantics. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar

  • Putnam, Hilary. 1975. The meaning of meaning. In K. Gunderson (ed.), Language, mind and knowledge, 131–193. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.Google Scholar

  • Randolph, Mary C. & Barbara B. Brooks. 1967. Conditioning of a vocal response in a chimpanzee through social reinforcement. Folia Primatologica 5. 70–79.Google Scholar

  • Salzinger, Kurt & Marcus B. Waller. 1962. The operant control of vocalization in the dog. Journal of the Experimental Analysis 5. 383–389.Google Scholar

  • Schel, Anne Marijke, Simon W. Townsend, Zarin Machanda, Klaus Zuberbühler & Katie E. Slocombe. 2013. Chimpanzee alarm call production meets key criteria for intentionality. PLoS One 8. e76674.Google Scholar

  • Scott-Phillips, Thom C. 2014. Speaking our minds. New York: Palgrave Macmillan.Google Scholar

  • Seyfarth, Robert M., Dorothy L. Cheney & Thore J. Bergman. 2005. Primate social cognition and the origins of language. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 9. 264–266.Google Scholar

  • Seyfarth, Robert M., Dorothy L. Cheney & Peter Marler. 1980a. Monkey responses to three different alarm calls: Evidence of predator classification and semantic communication. Science 210. 801–803.Google Scholar

  • Seyfarth, Robert M., Dorothy L. Cheney & Peter Marler. 1980b. Vervet monkey alarm calls: semantic communication in a free-ranging primate. Animal Behaviour 28. 1070–1094.Google Scholar

  • Seyfarth, Robert M. & Dorothy L. Cheney. 1997. Behavioral mechanisms underlying vocal communication in nonhuman primates. Animal Learning and Behavior 25. 249–267.Google Scholar

  • Seyfarth, Robert M. & Dorothy L. Cheney. 2003. Signalers and receivers in animal communication. Annual Review of Psychology 54. 145–173.Google Scholar

  • Sperber, Dan & Dierdre Wilson. 1986. Relevance: Communication and cognition. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar

  • ten Cate, Carel & Kazuo Okanoya. 2012. Revisiting the syntactic abilities of non-human animals: natural vocalizations and artificial grammar learning. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B 367. 1984–1994.Google Scholar

  • Tomasello, Michael. 2007. If they’re so good at grammar, then why don’t they talk? Hints from apes’ and humans’ use of gestures. Language Learning & Development 3. 133–156.Google Scholar

  • van Heijningen, Caroline A.A., Jos de Vissera, Willem Zuidema & Carel ten Cate. 2009. Simple rules can explain discrimination of putative recursive syntactic structures by a songbird species. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 106. 20538–20543.Google Scholar

  • Wanker, Ralf, Yasuko Sugama & Sabine Prinage. 2005. Vocal labelling of family members in spectacled parrotlets, Forpus conspicullatus. Animal Behavior 70. 111–118.Google Scholar

  • Watson, Stuart K., Simon W. Townsend, Anne M. Schel, Claudia Wilke, Emma K. Wallace, Leveda Cheng, Victoria West & Katie E. Slocombe. 2015. Vocal learning in the functionally referential food grunts of chimpanzees. Current Biology 25. 495–499.Google Scholar

  • Wheeler, Brandon C. & Julia Fischer. 2012. Functionally referential signals: A promising paradigm whose time has passed. Evolutionary Anthropology 21. 195–205.Google Scholar

  • Wilson, William A. Jr. 1975. Discriminative conditioning of vocalizations in Lemur catta. Animal Behavior 23. 432–436.Google Scholar

  • Winter, Peter, Patricia Handley, Detlev Ploog & Ditmar Schott. 1973. Ontogeny of squirrel monkey calls under normal conditions and under acoustic isolation. Behaviour 47. 230–239.Google Scholar

  • Yip, Moira J. 2006. The search for phonology in other species. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 10. 442–446.Google Scholar

About the article

Published Online: 2016-07-05

Published in Print: 2016-07-01


Citation Information: Theoretical Linguistics, ISSN (Online) 1613-4060, ISSN (Print) 0301-4428, DOI: https://doi.org/10.1515/tl-2016-0003.

Export Citation

©2016 by De Gruyter Mouton. Copyright Clearance Center

Citing Articles

Here you can find all Crossref-listed publications in which this article is cited. If you would like to receive automatic email messages as soon as this article is cited in other publications, simply activate the “Citation Alert” on the top of this page.

[1]
Rhodora Beaton
Horizons, 2017, Volume 44, Number 01, Page 56

Comments (0)

Please log in or register to comment.
Log in