Jump to ContentJump to Main Navigation
Show Summary Details
More options …

The Linguistic Review

Editor-in-Chief: Hulst, Harry

4 Issues per year


IMPACT FACTOR 2016: 0.676
5-year IMPACT FACTOR: 0.831

CiteScore 2016: 0.52

SCImago Journal Rank (SJR) 2015: 0.662
Source Normalized Impact per Paper (SNIP) 2015: 0.573

Online
ISSN
1613-3676
See all formats and pricing
More options …
Volume 34, Issue 3 (Aug 2017)

Issues

External syntax and the Cumulative Effect in subject sub-extraction: An experimental evaluation

Ciro Greco / Marco Marelli / Liliane Haegeman
Published Online: 2017-07-04 | DOI: https://doi.org/10.1515/tlr-2017-0009

Abstract

In this paper, we test the Cumulative Effect proposed by (Haegeman et al. 2014. Deconstructing the subject condition in terms of cumulative constraint violation. The Linguistic Review 31(1). 73–150). In particular, we focus on the role of factors of external syntax in the modulation of subject opacity to extraction, addressing different constraints presented in (Haegeman et al. 2014. Deconstructing the subject condition in terms of cumulative constraint violation. The Linguistic Review 31(1). 73–150). Two sets of formal acceptability judgments are presented. In the first experiment, we address the opacity of subjects from a broad point of view, in order to assess whether subject DPs are more resistant to extraction than other DPs. The results confirm that subject constituents are more opaque to extraction than object constituents. In the second experiment, we address the impact on the modulation of the Cumulative Effect of three different constraints individually: the Freezing Principle, the Inactivity Condition and the Edge Condition. We did that through the manipulation of two different factors: the position of the extraction site and the predicate type. The results do not confirm the predictions made by the Cumulative Effect, since the interaction between different factors does not appear to be additive and incremental. We discuss the implications of these results for the existing theories of subject islands and address an alternative perspective, recently proposed by (Bianchi, Valentina & Cristiano Chesi. 2014. Subject islands, reconstruction, and the flow of the computation. Linguistic Inquiry 45(4). 525–569), according to which the opacity of subjects is a function of the syntax semantics interface.

Keywords: experimental syntax; subject islands; sub-extraction; grammaticality gradience; Italian

References

  • Alexiadou, Artemis & Elena Anagnostopoulou. 2001. The subject-in-situ generalization and the role of case in driving computations. Linguistic inquiry 32(2). 193–231.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Alexiadou, Artemis & Elena Anagnostopoulou. 2007. The subject-in-situ generalization revisited. In Gärtner & Sauerland (eds.), Interfaces + Recursion =Language?, 31–60. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar

  • Alrenga, Peter. 2005. A sentential subject asymmetry in English and its implications for complement selection. Syntax 8. 175–207.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Baayen, R. Harald, Douglas J. Davidson & Douglas M. Bates. 2008. Mixed-effects modeling with crossed random effects for subjects and items. Journal of Memory and Language 59(4). 390–412.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bach, Edmond. 1986. The algebra of events. Linguistics and Philosophy 9(1). 5–16.Google Scholar

  • Bader, Markus & Jana Häussler. 2010. Toward a model of grammaticality judgments. Journal of Linguistics 46. 273–330.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bard, E. G., Dan Robertson & Antonella Sorace. 1996. Magnitude estimation of linguistic acceptability. Language 72. 32–68.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Belletti, Adriana. 1988. The case of unaccusatives. Linguistic inquiry 19. 1–34.Google Scholar

  • Belletti, Adriana. 2001. Inversion as focalization. In Aa e Hulk & Jean-Yves Pollock (eds.), Subject inversion in Romance and the theory of Universal Grammar, 60–90. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar

  • Belletti, Adriana. 2004. Aspects of the low IP area. In Luigi Rizzi (ed.), The structure of CP and IP, 16–51. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar

  • Bianchi, Valentina & Cristiano Chesi. 2014. Subject islands, reconstruction, and the flow of the computation. Linguistic Inquiry 45(4). 525–569.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bianchi, Valentina & Cristiano Chesi. 2015. On a PP/DP asymmetry in extraction. In Di Domenico, Hamann & Matteini (eds), Structures, strategies and beyond. Studies in honour of Adriana Belletti, 47–66. Amsterdam and New York: John Benjamins.Google Scholar

  • Boeckx, Cedric. 2003. Islands and chains. Stranding as resumption. Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing.Google Scholar

  • Bowers, John S. 2010. Arguments as relations. Cambridge: MA. MIT Press.Google Scholar

  • Broekhuis, Hans. 2006. Extraction from subjects: Some remarks on Chomsky’s “On Phases”. In Hans Broekhuis, Norbert Corver & Riny Hybreghts (ed.), Organising grammar, 59–68. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar

  • Burzio, Luigi. 1981. Intransitive verbs and Italian auxiliaries. Ph.D. Thesis. MIT.Google Scholar

  • Cardinaletti, Anna. 2004. Towards a cartography of subject positions. In Rizzi (ed.), The structure of CP and IP: The cartography of syntactic structures, 2, 115–165. New York: Oford University Press.Google Scholar

  • Carlson, Gregory Norman, Reference to kinds in English. 1977. Doctoral Dissertations, University of Massachusetts Amherst. Available from Proquest. AAI7726414. http://scholarworks.umass.edu/dissertations/AAI7726414.

  • Chaves, Rui. 2013. An expectation-based account of subject islands and parasitism. Journal of Linguistics 49(2). 285–327.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Chomsky, Noam. 1973. Conditions on transformations. In Anderson & Kiparsky (eds.), A Festschrift for Morris Halle, 232–286. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.Google Scholar

  • Chomsky, Noam. 1977. Essays on form and interpretation. New York: North-Holland.Google Scholar

  • Chomsky, Noam. 1981. Lectures on government and binding. Foris: Dordrecht.Google Scholar

  • Chomsky, Noam. 1986. Barriers. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar

  • Chomsky, Noam. 1995. The minimalist program, vol. 1765. Cambridge, MA: MIT press.Google Scholar

  • Chomsky, Noam. 2000. Minimalist inquiries. In Michaels Martin & Uriagereka (eds.), Step by step: Essays on minimalist syntax in honor of Howard Lasnik, 89–155. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar

  • Chomsky, Noam. 2001. Derivation by phase. In Michael Kenstowicz (ed.), Ken Hale: A life in language, 1–52. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar

  • Chomsky, Noam. 2008. On phases. In Peregrín Otero Freidin & Zubizarreta (eds.), Foundational issues in linguistic theory. Essays in Honor of Jean-Roger Vergnaud, 133–166. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar

  • Cinque, Guglielmo. 1977. The movement nature of left dislocation. Linguistic inquiry 8(2). 397–412.Google Scholar

  • Cinque, Guglielmo. 1980. On extraction from NP in Italian. Journal of Italian Linguistics 5(12). 47–99.Google Scholar

  • Cinque, Guglielmo. 1990. Types of Ā-dependencies. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar

  • Cinque, Guglielmo. 2012. Extraction from DP in Italian revisited. Ms. University.Google Scholar

  • Cohen, Jacob. 1988. Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences, 2nd ed. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar

  • Collins, Chris. 2005. A smuggling approach to the passive in English. Syntax 8. 81–120.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Davies, William D. & Stanley Dubinsky. 2003. On extraction from NPs. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 21. 1–37.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Delahunty, Gerald P. 1983. But sentential subjects do exist. Linguistic Analysis 12. 379–398.Google Scholar

  • Emonds, Joseph 1976. A transformational approach to English syntax. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar

  • Erteschik-Shir, Nomi. 1981. On extraction from noun phrases (picture noun phrases). In Adriana Belletti, Luciana Brandi & Luigi Rizzi (eds.), Theory of markedness in Generative Grammar; Proceedings of the 4th GLOW Conference, 147–169. Pisa: Scuola Normale Superiore.Google Scholar

  • Fiengo, Robert & James Higginbotham. 1981. Opacity in NP. Linguistic Analysis 7. 347–373.Google Scholar

  • Gallego, Ángel. 2010. Phase theory 152. Amterdam: John Benjamins Publishing.Google Scholar

  • Gallego, Ángel J. & Juan Uriagereka. 2007. Sub-extraction from subjects: A phase theory account. Amsterdam studies in the theory and history of linguistic science series 4, 287.Google Scholar

  • Giorgi, Alessandra & Giuseppe Longobardi. 1991. The syntax of noun phrases: Configuration, parameters and empty categories. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar

  • Greco, C. 2014. Arguments and subjects in A’-syntax. Doctoral Dissertation. University of Milan-Bicocca.Google Scholar

  • Grimshaw, Jane. 1990. Argument Structure. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar

  • Haegeman, Liliane, Ángel Jiménez-Fernández & Andrew Radford. 2014. Deconstructing the subject condition in terms of cumulative constraint violation. The Linguistic Review 31(1). 73–150.Google Scholar

  • Horn, George M. 1974. The noun phrase constraint. Ph.D. Thesis. University of Massachusetts.Google Scholar

  • Huang, C-T. James. 1982. Logical relations in Chinese and the theory of grammar. Ph.D. Thesis. MIT.Google Scholar

  • Jiménez Fernández, Ángel. 2009. On the composite nature of subject islands: A phase‐based approach. Sky Journal of Linguistics 22.Google Scholar

  • Jurka, Johannes. 2009. Gradient acceptability and subject islands in German. Ms., University of Maryland.Google Scholar

  • Jurka, Johannes. 2010. The importance of being a complement: CED effects revisited. Ph.D. Thesis. University of Maryland.Google Scholar

  • Jurka, Johannes, Chizuru Nakao & Akira Omaki. 2011 It’s not the end of the CED as we know it: Revisiting German and Japanese subject Islands. Proceedings of the 28th West Coast Conference on Formal Linguistics.Google Scholar

  • Keller, Frank. 2000. Gradience in grammar. Experimental and computational aspects of degrees of grammaticality. Edinburgh: University of Edinburgh dissertation.Google Scholar

  • Kitagawa, Yoshihisa. 1986. Subjects in English and japanese. Amherst, MA: University of Massachusetts dissertation.Google Scholar

  • Kluender, Robert. 1998. On the distinction between strong and weak islands: A processing perspective. In Culicover & McNally (eds.), Syntax and semantics 29: The limits of syntax, 241–279. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar

  • Kluender, Robert. 2004. Are subject Islands subject to a processing account? In Chand, Kelleher, Rodriguez & Schmeiser (eds.), Proceedings of the West Coast Conference on Formal Linguistics 23, 101–125. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar

  • Koopman, Hilda & Dominique Sportiche. 1991. The position of subjects. Lingua 85(2). 211–258.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Koster, Jan. 1978. Why subject sentences don’t exist. In Samuel Jay Keyser (ed.), Recent transformational studies in European languages, 53–64. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar

  • Kratzer, Angelika. 1996. Severing the external argument from its verb. In Rooryck & Zaring (eds.), Phrase structure and the Lexicon, 109–137. Dordrecht: Kluwer.Google Scholar

  • Kuno, Susumu. 1973. Constraints on internal clauses and sentential subjects. Linguistic Inquiry 4. 363–385.Google Scholar

  • Kuroda, S.-Y. 1972. The categorical and the thetic judgment: Evidence from Japanese syntax. Foundations of Language 9. 153–185.Google Scholar

  • Ladusaw, William. 1994. Thetic and categorical, stage and individual, weak and strong. In Harvey & Santelmann (eds.), Proceedings of the 4th Semantics and Linguistic Theory Conference (SALT), 220–229.Google Scholar

  • Lasnik, Howard & Myung-Kwan Park. 2003. The EPP and the subject condition under sluicing. Linguistic Inquiry 34. 649–660.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Law, Paul. 1998. A unified analysis of P-Stranding in Romance and Germanic. In Pius Tamanji & Kiyomi Kusumoto (eds.), Proceedings of the North Eastern Linguistic Society 28, 219–234. Amherst: Graduate Linguistics Student Association.Google Scholar

  • Law, Paul. 2006. Preposition stranding. The Blackwell companion to syntax, 631–684.Google Scholar

  • Lohndal, Terje. 2011. Freezing effects and objects. Journal of Linguistics 47. 163–199.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Longobardi, Giuseppe. 2000. Postverbal subjects and the mapping hypothesis. Linguistic Inquiry 31(4). 691–702.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • May, Robert. 1977. The grammar of quantification. Ph.D. Thesis. MIT.Google Scholar

  • McCloskey, Jim. 1997. Subjecthood and subject positions. In Haegeman (ed.), Elemenst of grammar, 197–236. Netherlands: Springer.Google Scholar

  • Milsark, Gary. 1974. Existential sentences in English. Ph.D. Thesis. MIT.Google Scholar

  • Moro, Andrea. 1997. The raising of predicates: Predicative noun phrases and the theory of clause structure. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar

  • Nuñes, Juan & Juan Uriagereka. 2000. Cyclicity and extraction domains. Syntax 3. 20–43.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Ordóñez, Francisco. 1998. Postverbal asymmetries in Spanish. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 16. 313–346.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Pinto, Manuela. 1997. Licensing and interpretation of inverted subjects in Italian. UiL OTS Dissertation Series. Utrecht.Google Scholar

  • Polinsky, M, CG Gallo, P Graff, E Kravtchenko, AM Morgan & A. Sturgeon. 2013. Subject Island are different. In Sprouse (ed.), Experimental syntax and Island effects. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar

  • Postal, Paul M. 1974. On raising. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar

  • Postal, Paul M. 1998. Three investigations of extraction. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press.Google Scholar

  • Rackowski, Andrea & Norvin Richards. 2005. Phase edge and extraction: A Tagalog case study. Linguistic Inquiry 36(4). 565–599.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Riemsdijk, Henk C. van. 1978. A case study in syntactic markedness: The binding nature of prepositional phrases. Dordrecht: Foris.Google Scholar

  • Rizzi, Luigi. 1982. Issues in Italian syntax. Dordrecht: Foris.Google Scholar

  • Rizzi, Luigi. 1997. The Fine Structure of the Left Periphery. In L. Haegeman, (ed.), Elements of grammar. Dordrecht: Kluwer.Google Scholar

  • Rizzi, Luigi. 2006. On the form of chains: Criterial positions and ECP e ects. In Lisa Cheng & Norvin Corver (eds.), Wh-Movement: Moving on, 97–133. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar

  • Rizzi, Luigi. 2015. Notes on labeling and subject positions. In Di Domenico, Hamann & Matteini (eds.), Structures, strategies and beyond. Studies in honour of Adriana Belletti, 17–46. Amsterdam and New York: John Benjamins.Google Scholar

  • Rizzi, Luigi & Ur. Shlonsky. 2007. Strategies of subject extraction. In H. M. Gärtner & U. Sauerland (eds.), Interfaces+Recursion=Language?, 115–160. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar

  • Ross, John Robert. 1967. Constraints on variables in syntax. Ph.D. Thesis. MIT.Google Scholar

  • Speas, Peggy. 1986. Adjunction and projections in syntax. Ph.D. Thesis. MIT.Google Scholar

  • Sportiche, Dominique. 1988. A theory of floating quantifiers and its corollaries for constituent structure. Linguistic Inquiry 19. 425–449.Google Scholar

  • Sprouse, Jon. 2011. A test of the cognitive assumptions of magnitude estimation: Commutativity does not hold for acceptability judgments. Language 87(2). 274–288.Google Scholar

  • Sprouse, Jon & Diogo Almeida. 2013. The empirical status of data in syntax: A reply to Gibson and Fedorenko. Language and Cognitive Processes 28(3). 222–228.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Sprouse, Jon, Ivano Caponigro, Ciro Greco & Carlo Cecchetto. 2015. Experimental syntax and the variation of island effects in English and Italian. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory.Google Scholar

  • Sprouse, Jon, Carson T. Schütze & Diogo Almeida. 2013. A comparison of informal and formal acceptability judgments using a random sample from Linguistic Inquiry 2001-2010. Lingua 134. 219–248.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Stepanov, Arthur. 2001. Cyclic domains in syntactic theory. Ph.D. Thesis. University of Connecticut.Google Scholar

  • Stepanov, Arthur. 2007. The end of CED? Minimalism and extraction domains. Syntax 10. 80–126.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Stowell, Tim. 1981. Origins of phrase structure. Ph.D. Thesis. MIT.Google Scholar

  • Suñer, Margarita. 1992. Two properties of clitics in clitic-doubled constructions. In Huang & May (eds.), Logical structure and linguistic structure: Cross-linguistic perspectives, 233–251. Dordrecht: Reidel.Google Scholar

  • Svenonius, Peter. 2002. Introduction. In Svenonius (ed.), Subjects, expletives, and the EPP. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar

  • Takahashi, Daiko. 1994. Minimality of movement. Ph.D. Thesis. University of Connecticut.Google Scholar

  • Tenny, Carol. 1989. The aspectual interface hypothesis. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press.Google Scholar

  • Uriagereka, Juan. 1999. Multiple spell-out. In Epstein & Hornstein (eds.), Working minimalism, 251–282. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar

  • Vicente, Luis. 2009. An alternative to remnant movement for partial predicate fronting. Syntax 12. 158–191.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Weskott, Thomas & Gisbert. Fanselow 2011. On the informativity of different measures of linguistic acceptability. Language 87. 249–273.CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wexler, Kenneth & Peter W. Culicover. 1980. Formal principles of language acquisition. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar

  • Zubizarreta, María Luisa. 1998. Prosody, focus and word order. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar

About the article

Published Online: 2017-07-04

Published in Print: 2017-08-28


Citation Information: The Linguistic Review, ISSN (Online) 1613-3676, ISSN (Print) 0167-6318, DOI: https://doi.org/10.1515/tlr-2017-0009.

Export Citation

© 2017 Walter de Gruyter GmbH, Berlin/Boston. Copyright Clearance Center

Comments (0)

Please log in or register to comment.
Log in