Jump to ContentJump to Main Navigation
Show Summary Details
More options …

Translational Neuroscience

Editor-in-Chief: David, Olivier

1 Issue per year


IMPACT FACTOR 2016: 0.922
5-year IMPACT FACTOR: 1.030

CiteScore 2016: 1.13

SCImago Journal Rank (SJR) 2015: 0.704
Source Normalized Impact per Paper (SNIP) 2015: 0.286

Open Access
Online
ISSN
2081-6936
See all formats and pricing
More options …

A touchy subject: advancing the modulated visual pathways account of altered vision near the hand

J. Eric T. Taylor / Davood G. Gozli / David Chan / Greg Huffman / Jay Pratt
Published Online: 2014-11-07 | DOI: https://doi.org/10.1515/tnsci-2015-0001

Abstract

A growing body of evidence demonstrates that human vision operates differently in the space near and on the hands; for example, early findings in this literature reported that rapid onsets are detected faster near the hands, and that objects are searched more thoroughly. These and many other effects were attributed to enhanced attention via the recruitment of bimodal visual-tactile neurons representing the hand and near-hand space. However, recent research supports an alternative account: stimuli near the hands are preferentially processed by the action-oriented magnocellular visual pathway at the expense of processing in the parvocellular pathway. This Modulated Visual Pathways (MVP) account of altered vision near the hands describes a hand position-dependent trade-off between the two main retinal-cortical visual pathways between the eye and brain. The MVP account explains past findings and makes new predictions regarding near-hand vision supported by new research.

Keywords: Magnocellular pathway; Action and perception; Attention

References

  • [1] Colby C. L., Goldberg M. E., Space and attention in parietal cortex, Ann. Rev. Neurosci., 1999, 22, 319-349 CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • [2] Duhamel J.-R., Bremmer F., BenHamed S., Graf W., Spatial invariance of visual receptive fields in parietal cortex neurons, Nature, 1997, 389, 845-848 Google Scholar

  • [3] Graziano M. S., Gross C. G., Spatial maps for the control of movement, Curr. Opin. Neurobiol., 1998, 8, 195-201 CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • [4] Brown L. E., Kroliczak G., Demonet J. F., Goodale M. A., A hand in blindsight: hand placement near target improves size perception in the blind visual field, Neuropsychologia, 2008, 46, 786-802 CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • [5] Serino A., Bassolino M., Farnè A., Làdavas E., Extended multisensory space in blind cane users, Psychol. Sci., 2007, 18, 642-648 CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • [6] Halligan P. W., Fink G. R., Marshall J. C., Vallar G., Spatial cognition: evidence from visual neglect, Trends Cogn. Sci., 2003, 7, 125-133 CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • [7] Rizzolatti G., Scandolara C., Matelli M., Gentilucci M., Afferent properties of periarcuate neurons in macaque monkeys. II. Visual responses, Behav. Brain Res., 1981, 2, 147-163 CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • [8] Graziano M. S., Gross C. G., A bimodal map of space: somatosensory receptive fields in the macaque putamen with corresponding visual receptive fields, Exp. Brain Res., 1993, 97, 96-109 CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • [9] Reed C. L., Grubb J. D., Steele C., Hands up: attentional prioritization of space near the hand, J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform., 2006, 32, 166 CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • [10] Iriki A., Tanaka M., Iwamura Y., Coding of modified body schema during tool use by macaque postcentral neurones, Neuroreport, 1996, 7, 2325-2330 CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • [11] Halligan P. W., Marshall J. C., Left neglect for near but not far space in man, Nature, 1991, 350, 498-500 Google Scholar

  • [12] Vuilleumier P., Valenza N., Mayer E., Reverdin A., Landis T., Near and far visual space in unilateral neglect, Ann. Neurol., 1998, 43, 406-410 CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • [13] Berti A., Frassinetti F., When far becomes near: remapping of space by tool use, J. Cogn. Neurosci., 2000, 12, 415-420 Google Scholar

  • [14] Ackroyd K., Riddoch M. J., Humphreys G. W., Nightingale S., Townsend S., Widening the sphere of influence: using a tool to extend extrapersonal visual space in a patient with severe neglect, Neurocase, 2002, 8, 1-12 CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • [15] Schendel K., Robertson L. C., Reaching out to see: arm position can attenuate human visual loss, J. Cogn. Neurosci., 2004, 16, 935-943 Google Scholar

  • [16] Smith D. T., Lane A. R., Schenk T., Arm position does not attenuate visual loss in patients with homonymous field deficits, Neuropsychologia, 2008, 46, 2320-2325 CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • [17] Tseng P., Bridgeman B., Juan C. H., Take the matter into your own hands: a brief review of the effect of nearby-hands on visual processing, Vision Res., 2012, 72, 74-77 Google Scholar

  • [18] Brockmole J. R., Davoli C. C., Abrams R. A., Witt J. K., The world within reach: effects of hand posture and tool use on visual cognition, Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci., 2013, 22, 38-44 CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • [19] Posner M. I., Cohen Y., Components of visual orienting, In: Bouma H., Bouwhuis D. G. (Eds.) Attention and performance X: Control of language processes, 1984, 32, 531-556 Google Scholar

  • [20] Reed C. L., Betz R., Garza J. P., Roberts R. J. Jr., Grab it! Biased attention in functional hand and tool space, Atten. Percept. Psychophys., 2010, 72, 236-245 CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • [21] Abrams R. A., Davoli C. C., Du F., Knapp W. H. 3rd, Paull D., Altered vision near the hands, Cognition, 2008, 107, 1035-1047 Google Scholar

  • [22] Vatterott D. B., Vecera S. P., Prolonged disengagement from distractors near the hands, Front. Psychol., 2013, 4, 533 CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • [23] Tseng P., Bridgeman B., Improved change detection with nearby hands, Exp. Brain Res., 2011, 209, 257-269 Google Scholar

  • [24] Davoli C. C., Brockmole J. R., The hands shield attention from visual interference, Atten. Percept. Psychophys., 2012, 74, 1386-1390 CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • [25] Davoli C. C., Brockmole J. R., Du F., Abrams R. A., Switching between global and local scopes of attention is resisted near the hands, Vis. Cogn., 2012, 20, 659-668 CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • [26] Cosman J. D., Vecera S. P., Attention affects visual perceptual processing near the hand, Psychol. Sci., 2010, 21, 1254-1258 CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • [27] Davoli C. C., Du F., Montana J., Garverick S., Abrams R. A., When meaning matters, look but don’t touch: the effects of posture on reading, Mem. Cognit., 2010, 38, 555-562 Google Scholar

  • [28] Gozli D. G., West G. L., Pratt J., Hand position alters vision by biasing processing through different visual pathways, Cognition, 2012, 124, 244-250 Google Scholar

  • [29] Brown L. E., Doole R., Malfait N., The role of motor learning in spatial adaptation near a tool, PLoS One, 2011, 6, e28999 Google Scholar

  • [30] Sternberg S., The discovery of processing stages: extensions of Donders’ method, Acta Psychol., 1969, 30, 276-315 CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • [31] Kao K. L. C., Goodale M. A., Enhanced detection of visual targets on the hand and familiar tools, Neuropsychologia, 2009, 47, 2454-2463 CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • [32] Posner M. I., Cohen Y., Rafal R. D., Neural systems control of spatial orienting, Phil. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B, 1982, 298, 187-198 Google Scholar

  • [33] Qiu F. T., Sugihara T., von der Heydt R., Figure-ground mechanisms provide structure for selective attention, Nat. Neurosci., 2007, 10, 1492-1499 CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • [34] Kimchi R., Peterson M. A., Figure-ground segmentation can occur without attention, Psychol. Sci., 2008, 19, 660-668 CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • [35] Livingstone M., Hubel D., Segregation of form, color, movement, and depth: anatomy, physiology, and perception, Science, 1988, 240, 740-749 Google Scholar

  • [36] Shapley R., Visual sensitivity and parallel retinocortical channels, Annu. Rev. Psychol., 1990, 41, 635-658 CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • [37] Goodale M. A., Milner A. D., Separate visual pathways for perception and action, Trends Neurosci., 1992, 15, 20-25 CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • [38] Mishkin M., Ungerleider L. G., Contribution of striate inputs to the visuospatial functions of parieto-preoccipital cortex in monkeys, Behav. Brain Res., 1982, 6, 57-77 CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • [39] Yeshurun Y., Isoluminant stimuli and red background attenuate the effects of transient spatial attention on temporal resolution, Vision Res., 2004, 44, 1375-1387 CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • [40] Yeshurun Y., Levy L., Transient spatial attention degrades temporal resolution, Psychol. Sci., 2003, 14, 225-231 CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • [41] Bocanegra B. R., Zeelenberg R., Emotion-induced trade-offs in spatiotemporal vision, J. Exp. Psychol. Gen., 2011, 140, 272-282 Google Scholar

  • [42] Pokorny J., Smith V. C., Psychophysical signatures associated with magnocellular and parvocellular pathway contrast gain, J. Opt. Soc. Am. A, 1997, 14, 2477-2486 CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • [43] Derrington A. M., Lennie P., Spatial and temporal contrast sensitivities of neurones in lateral geniculate nucleus of macaque, J. Physiol., 1984, 357, 219-240 Google Scholar

  • [44] Wiesel T. N., Hubel D. H., Spatial and chromatic interactions in the lateral geniculate body of the rhesus monkey, J. Neurophysiol., 1966, 29, 1115-1156 Google Scholar

  • [45] Kveraga K., Boshyan J., Bar M., Magnocellular projections as the trigger of top-down facilitation in recognition, J. Neurosci., 2007, 27, 13232-13240 CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • [46] Chan D., Peterson M. A., Barense M. D., Pratt J., How action influences object perception, Front. Psychol., 2013, 4, 462 CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • [47] Enns J. T., Di Lollo V., Object substitution: a new form of masking in unattended visual locations, Psychol. Sci., 1997, 8, 135-139 CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • [48] Goodhew S. C., Pratt J., Dux P. E., Ferber S., Substituting objects from consciousness: a review of object substitution masking, Psychon. Bull. Rev., 2013, 20, 859-877 CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • [49] Goodhew S. C., Gozli D. G., Ferber S., Pratt J., Reduced temporal fusion in near-hand space, Psychol. Sci., 2013, 24, 891-900 CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • [50] Goodhew S. C., Boal H. L., Edwards M., A magnocellular contribution to conscious perception via temporal object segmentation, J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform., 2014, 40, 948-959 CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • [51] Pokorny J., Review: steady and pulsed pedestals, the how and why of post-receptoral pathway separation, J. Vis., 2011, 11, 1-23 Google Scholar

  • [52] Goodhew S. C., Fogel N., Pratt J., The nature of altered vision near the hands: evidence for the magnocellular enhancement account from object correspondence through occlusion, Psychon. Bull. Rev., 2014, Epub ahead of print, DOI: 10.3758/s13423-014-0622-5 CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • [53] Hollingworth A., Franconeri S. L., Object correspondence across brief occlusion is established on the basis of both spatiotemporal and surface feature cues, Cognition, 2009, 113, 150-166 Google Scholar

  • [54] Barense M. D., Bussey T. J., Lee A. C., Rogers T. T., Davies R. R., Saksida L. M., et al., Functional specialization in the human medial temporal lobe, J. Neurosci., 2005, 25, 10239-10246 CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • [55] Barense M. D., Gaffan D., Graham K. S., The human medial temporal lobe processes online representations of complex objects, Neuropsychologia, 2007, 45, 2963-2974 CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • [56] Kahneman D., Treisman A., Gibbs B. J., The reviewing of object files: object-specific integration of information, Cogn. Psychol., 1992, 24, 175-219 CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • [57] Gozli D. G., Ardron J., Pratt J., Reduced visual feature binding in the near-hand space, Atten. Percept. Psychophys., 2014, 76, 1308-1317 CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • [58] Kimchi R., Yeshurun Y., Cohen-Savransky A., Automatic, stimulusdriven attentional capture by objecthood, Psychon. Bull. Rev., 2007, 14, 166-172 CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • [59] Doniger G. M., Foxe J. J., Murray M. M., Higgins B. A., Snodgrass J. G., Schroeder C. E., et al., Activation timecourse of ventral visual stream object-recognition areas: high density electrical mapping of perceptual closure processes, J. Cogn. Neurosci., 2000, 12, 615-621 Google Scholar

  • [60] Qian C., Al-Aidroos N., West G., Abrams R. A., Pratt J., The visual P2 is attenuated for attended objects near the hands, Cogn. Neurosci., 2012, 3, 98-104 CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • [61] Luck S. J., Hillyard S. A., Electrophysiological correlates of feature analysis during visual search, Psychophysiology, 1994, 31, 291- 308 CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • [62] Abrams R. A., Weidler B. J., Trade-offs in visual processing for stimuli near the hands, Atten. Percept. Psychophys., 2014, 76, 383-390 CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • [63] Breitmeyer B. G., Breier J. I., Effects of background color on reaction time to stimuli varying in size and contrast: inferences about human M channels, Vision Res., 1994, 34, 1039-1045 CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • [64] West G. L., Anderson A. K., Bedwell J. S., Pratt J., Red diffuse light suppresses the accelerated perception of fear, Psychol. Sci., 2010, 21, 992-999 CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • [65] Adam J. J., Bovend’Eerdt T. J., van Dooren F. E., Fischer M. H., Pratt J., The closer the better: hand proximity dynamically affects letter recognition accuracy, Atten. Percept. Psychophys., 2012, 74, 1533-1538 CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • [66] Taylor J. E. T., Witt J. K., Altered attention for stimuli on the hands, Cognition, 2014, 133, 211-225 Google Scholar

  • [67] Egly R., Driver J., Rafal R. D., Shifting visual attention between objects and locations: evidence from normal and parietal lesion subjects, J. Exp. Psychol. Gen., 1994, 123, 161-177 Google Scholar

  • [68] Bush W. S., Vecera S. P., Differential effect of one versus two hands on visual processing, Cognition, 2014, 133, 232-237 Google Scholar

  • [69] Dufour A., Touzalin P., Improved visual sensitivity in the perihand space, Exp. Brain Res., 2008, 190, 91-9Google Scholar

About the article

Received: 2014-09-18

Accepted: 2014-09-29

Published Online: 2014-11-07


Citation Information: Translational Neuroscience, ISSN (Online) 2081-6936, DOI: https://doi.org/10.1515/tnsci-2015-0001.

Export Citation

©2015 J Eric T Taylor et al.. This work is licensed under the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 3.0 License. BY-NC-ND 3.0

Citing Articles

Here you can find all Crossref-listed publications in which this article is cited. If you would like to receive automatic email messages as soon as this article is cited in other publications, simply activate the “Citation Alert” on the top of this page.

[1]
Merryn D. Constable, Jay Pratt, Davood G. Gozli, and Timothy N. Welsh
Visual Cognition, 2015, Volume 23, Number 6, Page 699

Comments (0)

Please log in or register to comment.
Log in